Eastern Gray Squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) Differ in Abundance and Response to Humans Across Urban Habitats of St. Louis
Liam D. Engel1, Elizabeth J. Carlen2, Jonathan B. Losos1, and Kristin M. Winchell1*
1Department of Biology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA. 2Department of Biology, Fordham University, Bronx, NY 10458, USA. *Corresponding author.
Urban Naturalist, No. 33 (2020)
Abstract
Behavioral responses to urbanization may differ with environmental variation across metropolitan areas. We compared relative abundance and escape behavior of Sciurus carolinensis (Eastern Gray Squirrels) in common urban habitats in St. Louis, Missouri, USA. We found that squirrels were abundant in urban park, forest, and neighborhood habitats and nearly absent in cemeteries and golf courses. Across all sites, we found abundance was positively associated with anthropogenic habitat modification: more people, impervious surface cover, roads, high intensity developed land use, and less canopy cover. Escape responses also varied across the urban landscape. Flight initiation distance, a metric of risk perception, was least in neighborhood and forest habitats and greatest in park and cemetery habitats. Most squirrels fled by sprinting a short distance and stopping in all habitat types, and those that ran quickly to a refuge tended to have greater flight initiation distances. Squirrels more readily fled in habitats with fewer roads and more residential land use. These findings suggest that responses to urbanization in Eastern Gray Squirrels are heterogeneous within the metropolitan region.
Download Full-text pdf
Site by Bennett Web & Design Co.
Urban
Naturalist
No. 33 2020
Eastern Gray Squirrels
(Sciurus carolinensis) Differ in
Abundance and Response
to Humans Across Urban Habitats
of St. Louis
Liam D. Engel, Elizabeth J. Carlen,
Jonathan B. Losos, and Kristin M. Winchell
Urban Naturalist
The Urban Naturalist (ISSN # 2328-8965) is published by the Eagle Hill Institute, PO Box 9, 59 Eagle Hill Road, Steuben, ME 04680-
0009. Phone 207-546-2821 Ext. 4, FAX 207-546-3042. E-mail: office@eaglehill.us. Webpage: http://www.eaglehill.us/urna. Copyright
© 2020, all rights reserved. Published on an article by article basis. Special issue proposals are welcome. The Urban Naturalist is
an open access journal. Authors: Submission guidelines are available at http://www.eaglehill.us/urna. Co-published journals: The
Northeastern Naturalist, Southeastern Naturalist, Caribbean Naturalist, and Eastern Paleontologist, each with a separate Board of
Editors. The Eagle Hill Institute is a tax exempt 501(c)(3) nonprofit corporation of the State of Maine (Federal ID # 010379899).
Board of Editors
Myla Aronson, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ,
USA
Joscha Beninde, University of California at Los Angeles,
CA, USA ... Editor
Sabina Caula, Universidad de Carabobo, Naguanagua,
Venezuela
Sylvio Codella, Kean University, Union New Jersey, USA
Julie Craves, University of Michigan-Dearborn, Dearborn,
MI, USA
Ana Faggi, Universidad de Flores/CONICET, Buenos
Aires, Argentina
Leonie Fischer, Technical University of Berlin, Berlin,
Germany
Chad Johnson, Arizona State University, Glendale, AZ,
USA
Kirsten Jung, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
Erik Kiviat, Hudsonia, Bard College, Annandale-on-
Hudson, NY, USA
Sonja Knapp, Helmholtz Centre for Environmental
Research–UFZ, Halle (Saale), Germany
David Krauss, City University of New York, New York,
NY, USA
Mark Laska, Great Ecology, consulting, La Jolla, CA, USA
Zdenka Lososova, Masaryk University, Brno, Czechia
Joerg-Henner Lotze, Eagle Hill Institute, Steuben, ME ...
Publisher
Kristi MacDonald, Hudsonia, Bard College, Annandale-on-
Hudson, NY, USA
Ian MacGregor-Fors, Insituto de Ecología Mexico,
Veracruz, Mexico
Tibor Magura, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
Brooke Maslo, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ,
USA
Mark McDonnell, Royal Botanic Gardens Victoria and
University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
Mike McKinney, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN,
USA
Desirée Narango, City University of New York, New York,
NY, USA
Mitchell Pavao-Zuckerman, University of Arizona, Tucson,
Arizona, USA
Joseph Rachlin, Lehman College, City University of New
York, New York, NY, USA
Travis Ryan, Center for Urban Ecology, Butler University,
Indianapolis, IN, USA
Michael Strohbach, Technische Universität Braunschweig,
Institute of Geoecology, Braunschweig, Germany
Katalin Szlavecz, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore,
MD, USA
Paige Warren, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA,
USA
Alan Yeakley, Portland State University, Portland, OR,
USA
Iriana Zuria, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Hidalgo,
Hidalgo, Mexico
♦ The Urban Naturalist is a peer-reviewed and
edited interdisciplinary natural history journal
with a global focus on urban areas (ISSN 2328-
8965 [online]).
♦ The journal features research articles, notes,
and research summaries on terrestrial, freshwater,
and marine organisms and their habitats.
♦ It offers article-by-article online publication
for prompt distribution to a global audience.
♦ It offers authors the option of publishing large
files such as data tables, and audio and video
clips as online supplemental files.
♦ Special issues - The Urban Naturalist welcomes
proposals for special issues that are based
on conference proceedings or on a series of invitational
articles. Special issue editors can rely
on the publisher’s years of experiences in efficiently
handling most details relating to the
publication of special issues.
♦ Indexing - As is the case with the Institute's
first 3 journals (Northeastern Naturalist, Southeastern
Naturalist, and Journal of the North
Atlantic), the Urban Naturalist is expected to
be fully indexed in Elsevier, Thomson Reuters,
Web of Science, Proquest, EBSCO, Google
Scholar, and other databases.
♦ The journal's staff is pleased to discuss ideas
for manuscripts and to assist during all stages of
manuscript preparation. The journal has a page
charge to help defray a portion of the costs of
publishing manuscripts. Instructions for Authors
are available online on the journal’s website
(http://www.eaglehill.us/urna).
♦ It is co-published with the Northeastern Naturalist,
Southeastern Naturalist, Caribbean Naturalist,
Eastern Paleontologist, Eastern Biologist,
and Journal of the North Atlantic.
♦ It is available online in full-text version on the
journal's website (http://www.eaglehill.us/urna).
Arrangements for inclusion in other databases
are being pursued.
Cover Photograph: An Eastern Gray Squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) in a park habitat in St. Louis, Missouri,
USA. Photograph © K.M. Winchell.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
1
2020 Urban Naturalist 33:1–16
Eastern Gray Squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis)
Differ in Abundance and Response to Humans
Across Urban Habitats of St. Louis
Liam D. Engel1, Elizabeth J. Carlen2, Jonathan B. Losos1, and Kristin M. Winchell1*
Abstract - Behavioral responses to urbanization may differ with environmental variation across metropolitan
areas. We compared relative abundance and escape behavior of Sciurus carolinensis (Eastern
Gray Squirrels) in common urban habitats in St. Louis, Missouri, USA. We found that squirrels were
abundant in urban park, forest, and neighborhood habitats and nearly absent in cemeteries and golf
courses. Across all sites, we found abundance was positively associated with anthropogenic habitat
modification: more people, impervious surface cover, roads, high intensity developed land use, and
less canopy cover. Escape responses also varied across the urban landscape. Flight initiation distance,
a metric of risk perception, was greatest in neighborhood and forest habitats and least in park and
cemetery habitats. Most squirrels fled by sprinting a short distance and stopping in all habitat types,
and those that ran quickly to a refuge tended to have greater flight initiation distances. Squirrels more
readily fled in habitats with fewer roads and more residential land use. These findings suggest that
responses to urbanization in Eastern Gray Squirrels are heterogeneous within the metropolitan region.
Introduction
Urban development rapidly transforms landscapes through increased impervious surface
cover, changes in vegetation type, novel landscape features, and decreased green space
(Grimm et al. 2008). Consequently, urbanization poses a threat to biodiversity by fragmenting
populations, reshaping predation pressures, and changing food availability (Ditchkoff
et al. 2006, McKinney 2008). Yet some species tolerate or even thrive in cities, exploiting
urban resources and, in some cases, adapting to urban environments (Ditchkoff et al. 2006,
Johnson and Munshi-South 2017). Clear morphological, behavioral, and physiological
differences exist between urban and non-urban populations of many species (reviewed in
Johnson and Munshi-South 2017). Yet, the extent to which variation in environmental features,
such as human presence and habitat structure, relate to variation within metropolitan
regions has been relatively understudied.
A wide variety of behavioral modifications have been documented in urban animals compared
to rural conspecifics (reviewed in Ditchkoff et al. 2006, Lowry et al. 2013). Tamias
striatus (Eastern Chipmunks) from urban habitats, for example, are more sedentary and have
lower fecal cortisol concentrations compared to those from non-urban habitats (Lyons et
al. 2017). Many urban behavioral shifts are related to risk assessment, escape strategy, and
boldness, likely in response to shifts in predation pressures and habituation (Fischer et al.
2012, Lowry et al. 2013). Modification of risk-taking behaviors may allow additional time
and energy for other behaviors, such as foraging, and thereby may increase fitness where
predation risk is reduced (Lima and Dill 1990, Sparkman et al. 2018). Urban-associated
behavioral traits, such as increased boldness, decreased neophobia, and decreased fear of
humans, have been documented in urban populations of lizard species (Intellagama lesueurii
[Australian Water Dragons], Baxter-Gilbert et al. 2019; Anolis sagrei [Brown Anoles],
1Department of Biology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA. 2Department of Biology,
Fordham University, Bronx, NY 10458, USA. *Corresponding author: kmwinchell@wustl.edu.
Manuscript Editor: Travis Ryan
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
2
Lapiedra et al. 2016). Similarly, in many taxa, particularly birds, urban individuals are less
wary of humans than non-urban conspecifics (reviewed in Lowry et al. 2013).
Behavioral responses to predation risk, such as fear responses and boldness, can provide
valuable insight into the decision-making processes of wildlife (Ydenberg and Dill
1986), as well as how specific environments shape behavior (Lowry et al. 2013). Flight
initiation distance (FID) is a commonly used metric for measuring perceived predation risk
(Stankowich and Blumstein 2005). FID measures how close a potential threat (commonly a
human or model predator) can approach a focal individual before eliciting a flight response
(Cooper et al. 2015), and is often used in studies examining optimal escape theory—which
predicts that animals flee when the risk of staying outweighs the cost of leaving (Ydenberg
and Dill 1986). Animals that appropriately assess and respond to the risks posed by potential
urban threats, in particular by humans, should be more likely to succeed in urban environments
(Samia et al. 2015). FID is, therefore, a particularly relevant metric for advancing our
understanding of which species are likely to tolerate increasing human presence and exploit
urban spaces where they will be exposed to novel selective pressures (Winchell et al. 2020).
Decreased FID does not necessarily correspond to decreased vigilance or fear in urban
animals. For example, escape distance in Brown Anoles decreased with urbanization, but
cautiousness increased (measured by latency to explore a novel environment), which was
attributed to increased ground predation (Lapiedra et al. 2016). Factors driving variable FID
may also differ between populations because of habituation, where a decreased behavioral
response to a repeated stimulus can influence the time and energy spent on anti-predator and
foraging behaviors (Chapman et al. 2012). Animals routinely exposed to human presence may
be less disturbed by them—allowing for closer approaches. Following this logic, researchers
have found a consistent pattern of decreasing FID with increasing human exposure in multiple
species of sciurids (Chapman et al. 2012, Cooper et al. 2008, Engelhardt and Weladji 2011,
Uchida et al. 2016) as well as birds (Carrete and Tella 2010, Ducatez et al. 2016).
Sciurus carolinensis (Eastern Gray Squirrels, Gmelin, 1788) are a particularly good focal
species for urban behavioral studies due to their synanthropic distribution and abundance in
both urban and non-urban habitats. This species is successful in urban environments and is
commonly habituated to human presence (Bateman and Fleming 2014, Cooper et al. 2008,
Engelhardt and Weladji 2011). Previous behavioral studies have examined relationships between
risk assessment behavior, such as FID, and a variety of ecological and environmental
factors. This body of research has established that squirrels flee at shorter distances (e.g.,
are less wary of human approach) in sites with higher levels of human activity (Cooper et
al. 2008), greater squirrel density (Parker and Nilon 2012), and distance to refuge (Dill and
Houtman 1989, but see Engelhardt and Weladji 2011). Furthermore, human behavior has
also been correlated with altered FID, with squirrels fleing more readily when humans approach
while looking directly at them (Bateman and Fleming 2014). Modified behavioral
responses in urban habitats depend on the interaction of perceived risk versus reward, which
may vary on fine scales with habitat features. For example, Bowers and Breland (1996)
found that squirrels were willing to forage longer (estimated by giving-up density, another
metric of risk assessment) at urban and suburban sites and in locations where the distances
to human structures were low, densities of squirrels were high, and trees were abundant.
In this study, we investigated the abundance and behavior of Eastern Gray Squirrels
across the St. Louis metropolitan area (Missouri, USA) to understand how behavioral responses
vary across distinct urban habitat types. We sampled 17 sites representing common
urban habitat types (parks, cemeteries, golf courses, residential neighborhoods, and urban
forest) that also varied in key habitat characteristics (number of people, canopy cover, perUrban
Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
3
cent impervious surface cover, roads, and land use). We addressed three main questions. We
first asked if squirrel abundance differs across the urban landscape. We predicted squirrels
would be more abundant in habitats such as forested areas and parks, which are more similar
to their natural habitat. We next asked how squirrel escape behavior varies across the urban
landscape. We predicted that squirrels in neighborhoods would have lower FID compared
to individuals from other habitat types, due to an increased exposure to humans. Finally,
we asked if differences in local and landscape-scale features were relevant to squirrel abundance
and escape behavior. We predicted that patterns of abundance and flight behavior
would be correlated with key ecological and environmental features across habitat types.
Specifically, we expected that abundances would be greatest and that squirrels would be
most wary (flee more readily and flee to refuge) in habitats with greater canopy cover, less
impervious surface, less developed land cover, and fewer people.
Field Site Description
St. Louis is a metropolitan region located along the Mississippi River in the states of
Missouri and Illinois in the United States (Fig. 1). The metropolitan region (metropolitan
statistical area) is a large, developed, and populous region with an estimated population of
2.8 million people in 2018, with over 300,00 residents in the central city of St. Louis, Missouri
(U.S. Census Bureau 2019). We sampled 17 sites distributed across approximately 70
km2 in the core metropolitan area in St. Louis City and County in eastern Missouri (Fig. 1).
We focused our sampling on five urban habitat types: golf courses, residential neighborhoods,
parks, cemeteries, and an urban forest. We chose these habitat types to capture
variation in both intensity of habitat modification (i.e., extent of impervious surface versus
tree cover and presence of anthropogenic structures) and human presence. The most heavily
modified sites were residential neighborhoods, which had many trees but discontinuous
tree canopy, with houses, impervious surfaces (streets, sidewalks), landscaped vegetation,
and a consistent presence of humans. Parks, golf courses, and cemeteries were intermediate
in disturbance, representing highly modified urban green-spaces that still had substantial
vegetation and tree cover but differed in human presence and activity. We sampled only
a single urban forest fragment (most urban forest fragments within our sample region resembled
park habitat more than forest), located in Forest Park. Forest Park is a large (>500
ha) urban green space with mixed land uses including golf courses, recreation areas, a zoo,
museums, and native forest fragments. The forest portion of the park sampled, located in the
south-west corner, had minimal disturbance (some paved walking paths), nearly continuous
tree canopy cover, and very few people.
Methods
Study Organism
The Eastern Gray Squirrel is a small arboreal mammal (38.0–52.5 cm average body length,
including tail; Koprowski 1994) that occurs natively throughout eastern North America and
has been introduced in Ireland, Italy, South Africa, and the United Kingdom (IUCN ISSG
2018). Eastern Gray Squirrels are found in mature woodlands with tree species that produce
winter-storable seeds as well as anthropogenic environments, including residential areas and
urban parks (Baker 1959, Nixon and Hansen 1987). In forest habitats, Eastern Gray Squirrel
densities have been reported as <1/ha (Healy and Welsh 1992, Uhlig 1957) and by the mid 20th
century, dwindling numbers outside of urban environments caused concern for the species’
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
4
persistence (Baker 1959). Conversely, squirrel densities in urban habitats are often greater
(Hein 1997). In urban parks, densities have been reported to range between 2–51.5/ha (Manski
et al. 1980, Parker and Nilon 2008), with densities on an urban university campus reported at
5/ha (Hein 1997). Estimates of home ranges of Eastern Gray Squirrels vary from 0.5–20.2 ha,
although territorial exclusion is unlikely and overlap in home ranges is common (Doebel and
McGinnes 1974, Don 1983, Thompson 1978). Specifically within urban parks, home ranges
have been reported between 1.90–4.52 ha (Tounzen et al. 2012). The species descends to the
ground frequently to forage and cache food. Peak activity is in summer and autumn. In autumn,
activity peaks approximately 2 hours after sunrise and 2–5 hours before sunset (Bakken
1959, Thompson 1977).
Field Methods
We conducted surveys in the autumn of 2018 (19 September – 1 December), alternating
habitat types so that all land-use types were sampled throughout the entire season. To account
for minor seasonal variation throughout the sampling period, we noted the weather condi-
0 12.5 25 50 km
Woody Wetland
Mixed Forest
Developed Open Space
Developed Med. Intensity
Developed High Intensity
Developed Low Intensity
Barren Land
Pasture / Crops
Evergreen / Deciduous Forest
Water / Herb. Wetland
Shrub-Scrub / Herbaceous
100%
0%
100%
0%
0 2.5 5 10km
Land Cover (NLCD) - St. Louis,MO, USA Impervious Surface
Canopy cover
Land Cover (NLCD)
Figure 1. The St. Louis, Missouri metropolitan region and associated land cover. Left: St. Louis city, county, and
surrounding areas with sampled area outlined (dotted rectangle) and NLCD land cover (NLCD v.2011; Homer
et al. 2015). Right: impervious surface cover (top), tree canopy cover (middle), and NLCD land cover (bottom)
for the sampled region with sample locations indicated by small black circles. NLCD land cover classes for both
land cover images are on the lower left.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
5
tions at each site and obtained the average air temperature for St. Louis for the date and time
sampled at each site from the National Weather Service (NOAA 2019). The same researcher
(LDE) performed all field surveys and FID tests. We conducted habitat surveys on days with
typical land use and human presence for each site type (e.g., no major landscaping activity
or public events).
We surveyed each site once, for approximately 1.5 hours, recording distance covered
(using Gaia GPS, Trailbehind Inc. 2009–2019), elapsed time, and number of people seen.
We followed established walking paths or sidewalks at each site where possible, walking
along a single linear path without revisiting any portions of the habitat. Although we did not
mark squirrels sampled, it is unlikely that we encountered the same squirrel more than once
during our single sampling period per site. We counted all squirrels we observed and noted
whether they were on the ground or on a structure, as well as if the structure was natural
(e.g., vegetation, trees) or anthropogenic (e.g., buildings, fences).
When we spotted a squirrel on the ground, we measured the distance between the squirrel
and the researcher (sight distance) because the distance at which a researcher begins
approaching has been shown to influence FID in Eastern Gray Squirrels (Engelhardt and
Weladji 2011). The researcher then walked toward the squirrel in a standardized approach
(arms raised above head at a slow and constant pace) at an angle where the researcher was
clearly visible to the squirrel. We measured the distance between the squirrel’s starting location
and the researcher when the squirrel began to move away from the observer (FID).
Sight distance and FID were both measured using a laser distance meter (GLM 20, Bosch).
After the squirrel fled, we measured the distance from the squirrel’s starting location to the
nearest vegetation the squirrel could use for escape (i.e., large enough to support the weight
of a squirrel) and noted the escape strategy used by the squirrel (“sprint and stop” versus
“run to refuge”). In addition, we recorded the number of people and estimated the percentage
of impervious surface within a 10 m radius from the squirrel’s starting location.
Landscape Analysis
We processed the following layers in ArcMap 10.6.1 (ESRI): National Land Cover Database
(NLCD) land cover, canopy cover, and impervious surface (NLCD v.2011; Homer
et al. 2015), roads (US Census Bureau 2017), squirrel locations, and linear path sampled.
We projected all data to a common coordinate system (NAD 1983 UTM Zone 15N). We
buffered each sampling path by a 40 m radius based on the minimum reported adult home
range size for Eastern Gray Squirrels of 0.5 ha (Doebel and McGinnes 1974). We resampled
NLCD rasters (land cover, canopy, and impervious surface) from 30 m2 cell size to 5 m2
using bilinear resampling for canopy cover and impervious surface and nearest neighbor
for land cover classes. For each site (defined by the buffered sample path), we obtained the
range, mean, and standard deviation of the continuous raster variables and percentage of
land cover types for the NLCD land cover. We also determined the linear distance of roads
within each buffer and standardized this metric by the linear distance travelled in each site.
We quantified a total of nine landscape variables that capture key differences in the
five habitat types: impervious surface and canopy cover, number of people observed, road
extent, and NLCD land cover categorizations of developed open space (NLCD 21), high
intensity residential (NLCD 22), commercial and industrial (NLCD 23), developed high
intensity (NLCD 24), and forest (NLCD 41–43, 90). We used linear discriminant analysis
(implemented with the “lda” function in R package MASS; Venables and Ripley 2002) of the
nine scaled landscape variables using habitat type (neighborhood, park, forest, golf course,
cemetery) as the grouping factor. We validated the accuracy of the analysis in discriminating
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
6
habitat types using the function “confusionMatrix” in the R package caret (Kuhn 2019). We
used ANOVA to determine if each linear discriminant (LD) differed by habitat type with
marginal means pairwise comparisons implemented with the emmeans R package to evaluate
which habitat levels differed from each other (Lenth 2017). We use linear discriminants
that differed by habitat type in subsequent analyses.
Abundance Analyses
We estimated relative abundance as the total number of squirrels observed per site divided
by the total area sampled (linear distance travelled with a buffer of 10 m, an estimate of how
far we could consistently observe squirrels from a sampling path). We analyzed abundance as
the total number of squirrels observed per site. We first verified that sites did not significantly
differ in average temperature or sampling effort (time elapsed or distance travelled) with
ANOVA. We then conducted a generalized linear model (GLM) with Poisson error distribution
for the number of squirrels by significant linear discriminants from the landscape analysis.
Sampling effort (ln-transformed distance travelled and amount of time spent sampling at
a site) were significantly correlated with abundance and so we included them as covariates.
We also conducted a generalized linear model with binomial error distribution for the fraction
of squirrels on trees versus the ground by significant linear discriminants from the landscape
analysis, with ln-transformed distance sampled as a covariate (temperature and sampling time
were not significantly related to this response and so were not included).
We next analyzed a generalized linear model of abundance by landscape features per site:
impervious surface and canopy cover, number of people, road extent, and NLCD land cover
categories developed open space, high intensity residential, commercial and industrial, and developed
high intensity. We did not include forest cover in the model because only two sites had
non-zero values for this variable. We again used a Poisson error distribution and included average
temperature and time spent sampling each site as covariates. We used bidirectional model
selection by Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) to reduce the model, implemented with the
function “step” in R base stats. We evaluated the significance of terms in all three models with
a likelihood ratio test (LRT) implemented with the “drop1” function in R base stats.
Behavior Analyses
We used a generalized linear model with binomial error distribution to analyze escape
strategies (“sprint and stop” versus “run to refuge”) with respect to the significant landscape
LDs and including sight distance as a covariate. We used a linear model of ln-transformed
FID by significant landscape LDs with average temperature and ln-transformed sight distance
as covariates.
We used a second binomial GLM to examine escape strategy (with ln-transformed sight
distance as a covariate) including the same seven landscape variables used in the abundance
models, as well as three on-the-ground measurements taken for each focal squirrel (number
of people and estimated impervious surface within 10 m of the squirrel, and distance from
the squirrel to the nearest tree). We did not include number of people observed per site but
instead included the number of people observed within 10 m of the squirrel. We also ran
a second linear model of ln-transformed FID (with ln-transformed sight distance and temperature
as covariates) with respect to these same 10 variables. For each model, we used
bidirectional model selection by AIC to reduce the model and type III ANOVA implemented
with the “drop1” function in R base stats to evaluate the significance of terms.
We completed all statistical analyses in R (R Core Team 2019). We used the following R
packages in our analyses and visualizations of results: ggplot2 (Wickham 2016), reshape2
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
7
(Wickham 2007), emmeans (Lenth 2017), Rmisc (Hope 2013), gridExtra (Baptiste 2017)
and dplyr (Wickham et al. 2019).
Results
Habitat Variation
We sampled 17 sites: five residential neighborhoods, five parks, three cemeteries, three
golf courses, and one urban forest. Sites varied in number of people, road extent, impervious
surface cover, canopy cover, and NLCD land cover classes. Our linear discriminant
analysis (LDA) had an overall accuracy of 1 (indicating complete discrimination based on
habitat type, with 95% CI of 0.805–1). The first three LDs significantly differed by habitat
type and together explained 99% of between-class variance (Table 1). These three axes
were thus included in subsequent analyses. LD1 clearly separated golf course, park, and
cemetery habitats (pairwise marginal means t-tests with α < 0.05; Fig. 2A). LD2 separated
Table 1. Linear Discriminant Analysis (LDA) of habitat types (cemetery “C”, forest “F”, golf course “GC”,
neighborhood “N”, park “P”) based on nine landscape variables (see methods for details). Between-class
variance explained by each linear discriminant (LD), results of ANOVA by habitat type for each LD, number
of groups discriminated and identity of groups based on pairwise marginal means t-tests with α=0.05
(habitat types within parentheses do not differ at α=0.05) .
Variance ANOVA ~ habitat N Groups Marginal Mean Groups
LD1 0.56 F 4, 12 = 51.425, p < 0.001 4 (GC), (P), (C), (N, F)
LD2 0.29 F 4, 12 = 26.802, p < 0.001 3 (N), (P, GC, C), (F)
LD3 0.14 F 4, 12 = 12.689, p = 0.003 2 (GC, C, N), (P)
LD4 0.01 F 4, 12 = 1.090, p = 0.405 1 (C, P, N, GC, F)
Cemetery Forest Golf Course Neighborhood Park
−4
0
4
8
−2 0 2
LD 3
LD 2
−4
0
4
8
−8 −4 0 4
LD 1
LD 2
A B
Figure 2. Linear Discriminant Analysis (LDA) of landscape variables by habitat type (Cemetery, Forest, Golf
Course, Neighborhood, and Park). Each habitat type is uniquely distinguished by a combination of LD1 and
LD2. Sites are plotted with respect to (A) LD1 and LD2, and (B) LD2 and LD3, with colors corresponding
to habitat type. Shaded boxes and dashed lines indicate which habitats are significantly distinguished by
LD1 and LD2 in A and B, respectively (see Table 1).
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
8
sites into three groups: neighborhood and forest habitats were at the extremes with a single
group formed by golf course, cemetery, and park habitats (Fig. 2B). LD3 divided sites into
two groups, with golf course, cemetery, and park habitats forming one group, park habitats
forming another, and forest habitat intermediate between the two groups (Fig. 2B).
Abundance
We observed 337 squirrels across all sites. Parks had the highest estimated average abundance
across all sites (32 squirrels/ha, range: 13–75 squirrels/ha), followed by the urban
forest site (6 squirrels/ha), residential neighborhoods (5 squirrels/ha, range: 6–25 squirrels/
ha), cemeteries (4 squirrels/ha, range: 1–9 squirrels/ha), and golf courses (<1 squirrels/ha,
range: 0–1 squirrels/ha). We observed only three squirrels in neighborhoods and one in a
cemetery using anthropogenic structures.
Time spent, distance covered, and average air temperature while sampling squirrel abundance
at each site did not differ between sites (ANOVA, time: F4, 12 = 1.030, p = 0.431; distance:
F4, 12 = 1.118, p = 0.393; temperature: F4, 12 = 0.514, p = 0.727). Total number of squirrels
observed was positively associated with sampling effort: time spent at each site (Poisson
GLM, z = 8.179, p < 0.001) and ln-transformed distance travelled (Poisson GLM, z = 7.891,
p < 0.001). Fraction of squirrels observed on trees versus on the ground was also correlated
with sampling effort (ln-transformed distance; Binomial GLM, z = -3.916, p < 0.001).
Accounting for these covariates, we found that squirrel abundance (number of squirrels
observed per site) and habitat use (observed on ground or tree) varied significantly across
the urban landscape (Fig. 3). Abundance was positively correlated with LD1 and LD3, and
negatively correlated with LD2 (likelihood-ratio test; LD1: χ = 4.197, p = 0.041; LD2: χ
= 5.953, p = 0.015; LD3: χ = 25.609, p < 0.001). This corresponds to higher abundances
in neighborhood, forest, and park habitats compared to cemetery and golf course habitats.
Fraction of squirrels observed on trees versus on the ground was negatively correlated
(i.e., more squirrels were found in trees) with LD1 only (likelihood-ratio test; χ = -2.141,
p = 0.032). The abundance by landscape features model revealed multiple significant
0
20
40
Abundance of Squirrels
C F GC N P C F GC N P
0
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
Ground Tree A B
Proportion Squirrels
Figure 3. Relative abundance of squirrels by habitat type across all habitat types. (A) The mean abundance
of squirrels observed plus and minus standard error per site for each habitat type (Cemetery: “C”, Forest:
“F”, Golf Course: “GC”, Neighborhood: “N”, and Park: “P”). (B) The fraction of squirrels across all sites
within a habitat type found on trees (or anthropogenic structures) vs. on the ground.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
9
positive associations between squirrel abundance and landscape features (Table 2). Higher
abundances were associated with more: people, impervious surface cover, roads, and high
intensity developed land. Higher abundances were also associated with less: canopy cover,
developed open space, residential land use, and commercial/industrial land use.
Escape Behavior
We measured escape behavior (escape strategy and FID) for 136 squirrels in park (n =
76), neighborhood (n = 41), forest (n = 10), and cemetery (n = 9) habitats. We were unable
to obtain any behavioral measurements on squirrels on golf courses because we observed
only a single squirrel across the three golf course sites sampled. Escape strategies and FID
were both correlated with ln-transformed sight distance (escape strategy: Binomial GLM,
z = 2.338, p = 0.019; FID: t-test, t = 11.423, df = 134, p < 0.001). Additionally, average air
temperature during sampling was also associated with FID (t-test, t = 3.897, df = 134, p <
0.001). We therefore included ln-transformed sight distance in both models and temperature
in the FID model.
Escape strategies were correlated with FID, but not with landscape LDs (likelihoodratio
test; FID: χ = 6.917, df = 1, p = 0.009). Across all habitat types, most squirrels
sprinted a short distance and stopped and 29% ran quickly to a tree or other refuge;
sprinting quickly to refuge was associated with greater FID (Fig. 4A). Escape by running
quickly to a refuge was associated with lesser road extent (likelihood-ratio test; χ =
9.932, df = 135, p = 0.002) and higher proportions of residential (likelihood-ratio test; χ
= 11.228, df = 135, p < 0.001) and high intensity developed land (likelihood-ratio test;
χ = 15.535, df = 135, p < 0.001; Table 3). FID was negatively correlated with landscape
LD1 only, indicating squirrels in cemetery and park habitats had longer FIDs compared
to those in neighborhood and forest habitats (Type III ANOVA, F 1, 130 = 7.077, p = 0.009;
Fig. 4B). FID across all sites was negatively correlated with impervious surface cover
(Type III ANOVA, F 1, 127 = 4.028, p = 0.047) and road extent (Type III ANOVA, F 1, 127 =
8.693, p = 0.004), and positively correlated with greater amounts of residential land use
(Type III ANOVA, F 1, 127 = 4.767, p = 0.031; Table 3).
Table 2. Full model for abundance by habitat characteristics for squirrels across all sites. Estimate (Est.) and
standard error (SE) are given with χ test statistic and significance (p-value). Significant terms (p < 0.05) are
shaded. The full model contained all significant terms and could not be reduced further.
Abundance Full Model (AIC: 119.48)
Est. SE χ p
Time sampled 2.74 0.32 92.010 <0.001
Distance sampled -2.14 0.27 79.673 <0.001
Number people 0.06 0.01 60.919 <0.001
Impervious 21.09 10.73 3.946 0.047
Canopy -2.68 1.27 4.586 0.032
Road extent 1.86 5.16 13.810 <0.001
Developed open -1.98 0.86 5.488 0.019
Residential -8.68 2.65 11.457 0.001
Commercial -33.37 9.09 14.183 <0.001
Developed high intensity 33.13 10.39 10.111 0.001
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
10
Discussion
Although behavioral shifts in response to urbanization are well documented in many
taxa, few studies have examined how these responses differ across various urban landscapes.
Here we analyzed abundance and escape behavior of a common urban mammal
across 17 sites and five different urban habitat types. We analyzed these responses with
respect to continuous linear discriminants that clearly separate habitat types based on multiple
landscape variables. In doing so, we are able to better understand how abundance and
behavior varies in distinct urban habitat spaces that differ along a continuum of co-varying
habitat features. We also analyze these responses with respect to habitat features to understand
how specific landscape characteristics relate to abundance and behavior across the
urban region regardless of habitat type.
Abundance
We expected squirrels to be most abundant in areas with extensive canopy cover, as
found in previous studies (Bonnington et al. 2013, Williamson 1983). Greater canopy cover
provides increased protection and refuges from urban predators and seed-bearing tree species
are important food sources (Powell 1982, Williamson 1983). Moreover, a radiotracking
study of Eastern Gray Squirrels in an urban forest found that squirrels consistently avoided
developed and open grassy portions of the habitat (Tounzen et al. 2012). To some extent,
our results support our prediction: we observed abundant squirrels in our forest site, albeit
fewer than in parks. However, cemeteries and golf courses, which were minimally impacted
by urbanization and had abundant tree canopy cover, had the lowest relative abundances of
squirrels. Our linear discriminant analysis supports this observation: abundance was correlated
with all three linear discriminants indicating the lowest abundances in golf course
and cemetery habitat types (Table 2).
Flight-initiation Distance (m)
B
Flight-initiation Distance (m)
A Refuge Short Sprint
0
10
20
0
10
20
C F N P
Figure 4. Flight initiation distance by discrete habitat type and by escape strategy. Boxes show mean and
1st and 3rd quantiles, with whiskers extending 1.5 times the inter-quartile range and black dots representing
outliers. (A) Flight initiation distance (FID) differed across habitats sampled. Note that although presented
as discrete categories here, we analyzed FID with respect to the linear discriminants (Table 1, Fig. 2). (B)
FID was associated with escape strategy: squirrels that ran to refuge had greater FID than squirrels that
sprinted a short distance and stopped in all habitat types (dots represent samples from each habitat type,
colored as in A).
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
11
Golf courses are important habitats for many urban taxa, supporting high abundances,
stable populations, and diverse communities (Colding and Folke 2008, Hodgkison et
al. 2007, Winchell and Gibbs 2016). A related species, the Sciurus niger avicennia (Big
Cypress Fox Squirrel), is common in golf course habitats in other urban regions, and cemeteries
are known to support high abundances of Eastern Gray Squirrels (Ditgen et al. 2007,
Thompson 1978). Eastern Gray Squirrels in St. Louis may not extensively use golf course
or cemetery habitats because of factors not measured here. For example, landscaping activity
may discourage squirrel presence by interfering with anti-predator behavior because of
decreased auditory cues or may decrease forage effectiveness by disturbing food resources
on the ground (Barber et al. 2010). Additional anthropogenic activities may also be a factor
Table 3. Full and reduced models for escape behavior and FID by habitat characteristics for squirrels across
all sites. Estimate (Est.) and standard error (SE) for each model term are given. We evaluated significance of
terms in the escape strategy model with a likelihood ratio test and in the FID model with type III ANOVA,
and report the relevant statistics, χ and F, and p-values. Significant terms (p < 0.05) are shaded.
Escape Strategy Full Model (AIC: 155.53) Reduced Model (AIC: 147.27)
Est. SE χ p Est. SE χ p
Sight distance 0.64 0.41 2.49 0.115 0.70 0.33 4.64 0.031
Impervious 9.35 54.19 0.03 0.864
Canopy 4.75 4.18 1.46 0.227
Road extent -2.23 2.14 1.16 0.282 -3.24 1.16 9.93 0.002
Dev. Open 2.37 3.15 0.56 0.456
Residential 4.00 12.91 0.10 0.754 5.70 1.87 11.23 0.001
Commercial -4.51 43.25 0.01 0.917
Dev. high intensity 92.58 52.45 4.74 0.030 109.05 50.17 15.54 0.001
People <10m -0.42 0.35 2.85 0.091 -0.34 0.26 2.65 0.104
Imperv. <10m 1.26 1.78 0.52 0.471
Nearest tree -0.06 0.07 0.64 0.423
FID Full Model (AIC: 192.95) Reduced Model (AIC: 188.57)
Est. SE F p Est. SE F p
Sight distance 0.81 0.09 84.05 <0.001 0.79 0.08 88.96 0.001
Temperature 0.01 0.01 1.02 0.314 0.01 0.00 2.61 0.109
Impervious -19.43 10.07 3.73 0.056 -17.79 8.87 4.03 0.047
Canopy -0.99 6.25 2.49 0.117 -1.02 0.60 2.88 0.092
Road extent -1.13 3.92 8.32 0.005 -1.10 3.73 8.69 0.004
Dev. Open -0.83 0.54 2.35 0.128 -0.72 0.50 2.08 0.151
Residential 5.12 2.31 4.92 0.028 4.65 2.13 4.77 0.031
Commercial 15.38 7.54 4.16 0.044 13.67 7.07 3.75 0.055
Dev. high intensity -1.46 8.97 0.03 0.871
People <10m -0.07 0.04 2.51 0.116 -0.07 0.04 2.92 0.090
Imperv. <10m -0.14 0.32 0.19 0.660
Nearest tree -0.02 0.01 1.26 0.263
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
12
across specific urban habitat types, such as the active culling of squirrels or the use of herbicides
and pesticides that could lead to accidental poisoning—although these possibilities
are purely speculative and require further investigation.
We observed the greatest abundances of squirrels in sites with the highest values of
linear discriminants LD1 (forest and neighborhood habitats) and LD3 (park habitats), and
lowest values of LD2 (forest habitat). Parks, neighborhoods, and forests drastically differ in
their landscape composition and human activity levels. Park and forest habitats have many
features that should promote large populations of squirrels (e.g., abundant tree cover), but
neighborhood habitats have features that should disfavor abundant populations (e.g., extensive
roads, impervious surface cover, and developed land). We interpret our result that
forest and neighborhood habitats support similarly high abundances with caution. Observed
abundance in the forest site may be underestimated if we failed to detect squirrels or may
simply be a peculiarity of the single site we sampled. The other habitat types we sampled
were significantly more open and detection was not likely an iss ue.
Neighborhoods and other urban environments commonly have readily available anthropogenic
food resources, such as bird feeders and anthropogenic waste, which often support
increased abundances of urban animals (Ditchkoff et al. 2006). We detected a significant
positive association between anthropogenic landscape features and abundance across all
sites suggesting greater abundances with greater human disturbance. Higher abundances
were associated with more people at a site, greater impervious surface, lower canopy cover,
more roads, and more high intensity land use (Table 2). Lower abundances were associated
with sites that had greater proportions of developed open space (i.e., recreational space),
and residential and commercial land uses. Although Williamson (1983) documented lower
abundances of Eastern Gray Squirrels in areas with extensive impervious surface and building
cover, Hein (1997) noted a pattern of greater densities of Eastern Gray Squirrels in urban
habitats. Abundances of urban species of birds and reptiles have also been shown to increase
with low to moderate levels of human density and decline only at very high levels of human
presence (de Andrade 2019, Tratalos et al. 2007). Despite the overall positive relationship
between observed numbers of squirrels and anthropogenic disturbance that we detected,
we also found a negative relationship with increased commercial/industrial land use. These
relationships suggest the intriguing possibility that there may be a degree of moderate anthropogenic
disturbance that supports elevated abundances of Eastern Gray Squirrels.
Escape Behavior
We found that escape behavior significantly differed across the urban landscape with respect
to our landscape linear discriminants as well as with specific habitat features. FID was
positively correlated with linear discriminant LD1, with squirrels in park habitats fleeing at
the greatest distance and squirrels in forests and neighborhoods allowing closer approach before
fleeing. We also found that squirrels allowed closer approach (decreased FID) in habitats
that had greater impervious surface coverage and road extent and less residential land use
(Table 3). Our results thus add to the growing number of studies finding decreased FID in
sciurids with increasing anthropogenic habitat modification (Chapman et al. 2012, Cooper et
al. 2008, Engelhardt and Weladji 2011, Uchida et al. 2016). Notably, the habitats that had the
highest abundances were the same in which squirrels had the shortest FID, a finding similar
to Parker and Nilon (2008), who found decreased wariness in squirrels at higher densities.
Although escape strategy (fleeing to refuge versus short sprint and stop) did not differ
across the urban landscape, escape strategy was correlated with specific landscape attributes.
Squirrels were more likely to flee towards a refuge in habitats that had fewer roads and greater
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
13
proportions of residential and high intensity urban land use (Table 3). Cooper et al. (2008)
similarly found that squirrels reacted to an approaching human at a closer distance in more
heavily disturbed and open environments. In more open environments such as these, where
the distance to refuge is likely greater, squirrels may be less likely to flee and more likely to
remain immobile. However, there is no clear consensus on the relationship between FID and
distance to refuge in the Eastern Gray Squirrel: some studies have found an increased FID
with greater distance to refuge (Dill and Houtman 1989), yet others found no effect (Engelhardt
and Weladji 2011). In this study, we find no effect of distance to nearest tree on FID nor
on escape strategy across all sites. The relationship between urban land cover features and
escape strategy is one avenue of future study in this system.
Variation in flight responses may be attributable to personality differences among squirrels
related to boldness and fear. We found that squirrels that ran a short distance then stopped were
also more likely to allow a closer approach (shorter FID) compared to those that ran towards
a refuge. In other words, bolder squirrels may allow closer approach and not run as far when
they flee. Squirrels that have a low propensity to flee may also occupy more urbanized landscapes
if bolder animals are more likely to occur in more anthropogenically disturbed habitats
as suggested by Lowry et al. (2013). If use of such habitat leads to increased interaction
with humans, fear and startle responses may be increasingly dampened through habituation
(Batabyal et al. 2017, Engelhardt and Weladji 2011, Fernández-Juricic et al. 2009). Future
studies may investigate this topic further by examining how innate behavioral traits influence
variation in flight responses with repeated exposure to anthropogenic stimuli.
Overall, we found that both abundance and behavioral responses related to urbanization
are heterogeneous across a large portion of the core St. Louis urban landscape and may reflect
variation in habituation and other behaviors at local scales. Our results highlight the importance
of sampling multiple urban land-use types to understand nuanced differences in species
responses across complex urban landscapes. Failure to do so may impact the magnitude of
behavioral differences observed in simplified urban-rural comparisons. Local habitat features
appear to affect urban squirrel behavior, suggesting that FID and habituation may vary in
predictable ways among individuals inhabiting different types of urban habitats and even different
portions of a habitat. Behavioral modifications in response to human presence can alter
immune responses, stress levels, and reproductive success in urban animals, with implications
for their persistence and well-being (Atwell et al. 2012, French et al. 2008, Gotanda et al.
2009, Lyons et al. 2017). Individuals that properly assess the risk of humans as a threat and
respond appropriately with decreased FID will likely have increased fitness in these populations,
especially as urbanization intensifies and human population increases.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the honors program at Washington University in St. Louis and the Living
Earth Collaborative for supporting this undergraduate-led research project. We thank C. Donihue for
helpful discussions about urbanization, methods, and urban squirrels. We are grateful for the constructive
feedback from two anonymous reviewers, which significantly improved this manuscript.
Literature Cited
Atwell, J.W., G.C. Cardoso, D.J. Whittaker, S. Campbell-Nelson, K.W. Robertson, and E.D. Ketterson.
2012. Boldness behavior and stress physiology in a novel urban environment suggest rapid
correlated evolutionary adaptation. Behavioral Ecology 23:960–9 69.
Baker, R.H. 1959. The gray squirrel: Past, present, and future. Proceedings of the Southeastern Association
of Game and Fish Commissioners 13:390–392.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
14
Bakken, A. 1959. Behavior of Gray Squirrels. Proceedings of the Annual Conference, Southeastern
Association of Game and Fish Commissioners 13:393–406.
Baptiste, A. 2017. gridExtra: Miscellaneous Functions for “Grid” Graphics. R package version
2.3.https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gridExtra
Barber, J.R., K.R. Crooks, and K.M. Fristrup. 2010. The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial
organisms. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 25:180–189.
Batabyal, A., S. Balakrishna, and M. Thaker. 2017. A multivariate approach to understanding shifts in
escape strategies of urban lizards. Behavioral Ecology and Soci obiology 71:83.
Bateman, P.W., and P.A. Fleming. 2014. Does human pedestrian behavior influence risk assessment in
a successful mammal urban adapter? Journal of Zoology 294:93–98 .
Baxter-Gilbert, J., J.L. Riley, and M.J. Whiting. 2019. Bold new world: Urbanization promotes an
innate behavioral trait in a lizard. Behavioral Ecology and Soc iobiology 73:105.
Bonnington, C., K.J. Gaston, and K.L. Evans. 2013. Squirrels in suburbia: Influence of urbanisation
on the occurrence and distribution of a common exotic mammal. Urban Ecosystems 17:533–546.
Bowers, M.A., and B. Breland. 1996. Foraging of gray squirrels on an urban-rural gradient: Use of the
GUD to assess anthropogenic impact. Ecological Applications 6:1135–1142.
Carrete, M., and J. Tella. 2010. Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls:
A new hypothesis on disturbance-induced habitat selection. Biolo gy Letters 6:167–170.
Chapman, T., T. Rymer, and N. Pillay. 2012. Behavioural correlates of urbanisation in the Cape ground
squirrel Xerus inauris. Naturwissenschaften 99:893–902.
Colding, J., and C. Folke. 2008. The role of golf courses in biodiversity conservation and ecosystem
management. Ecosystems 12:191–206.
Cooper, C.A., A.J. Neff, D.P. Poon, and G.R. Smith. 2008. Behavioral responses of eastern gray squirrels
in suburban habitats differing in human activity levels. Northeastern Naturalist 15:619 –625.
Cooper, W.E., D.S.M. Samia, and D.T. Blumstein. 2015. Fear, spontaneity, and artifact in economic
escape theory: A review and prospectus. Pp. 147–175, In Advances in the Study of Behavior.
Academic Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. 552 pp.
de Andrade, A.C. 2019. Metropolitan lizards? Urbanization gradient and the density of lagartixas
(Tropidurus hispidus) in a tropical city. Ecology and Evolution. DOI: 10.1002/ece3.5518
Dill, L.M., and R. Houtman. 1989. The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in
the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 67:233–235.
Ditchkoff, S.S., S.T. Saalfeld, and C.J. Gibson. 2006. Animal behavior in urban ecosystems: Modifications
due to human-induced stress. Urban Ecosystems 9:5–12.
Ditgen, R.S., J.D. Shepherd, and S.R. Humphrey. 2007. Big cypress fox squirrel (Sciurus niger avicennia)
diet, activity and habitat use on a golf course in southwest Florida. American Midland
Naturalist 158:403–414.
Doebel, J.H., and B.S. McGinnes. 1974. Home range and activity of a gray squirrel population. The
Journal of Wildlife Management 38:860–867.
Don, B.A.C. 1983. Home range characteristics and correlates in tree squirrels. Mammal Review
13:123–132.
Ducatez, S., J.N. Audet, J.R. Rodriguez, L. Kayello, and L. Lefebvre. 2016. Innovativeness and
the effects of urbanization on risk-taking behaviors in wild Barbados birds. Animal Cognition
20:33–42.
Engelhardt, S.C., and R.B. Weladji. 2011. Effects of levels of human exposure on flight initiation
distance and distance to refuge in foraging eastern gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis). Canadian
Journal of Zoology 89:823–830.
Fernández-Juricic, E., E.F. Zahn, T. Parker, and T. Stankowich. 2009. California’s endangered Belding’s
savannah sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis beldingi): Tolerance of pedestrian disturbance.
Avian Conservation and Ecology 4:1.
Fischer, J.D., Cleeton, S.H., and T.P. Lyons. 2012. Urbanization and the predation paradox: The role
of trophic dynamics in structuring vertebrate communities. Bios cience 62:809–818.
French, S.S., H.B. Fokidis, and M.C. Moore. 2008. Variation in stress and innate immunity in the tree
lizard (Urosaurus ornatus) across an urban–rural gradient. Journal of Comparative Physiology B
178:997–1005.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
15
Gotanda, K.M., K. Turgeon, and D.L. Kramer. 2009. Body size and reserve protection affect flight
initiation distance in parrotfishes. Behavioral Ecology and Soci obiology 63:1563–1572.
Grimm, N.B., S.H. Faeth, N.E. Golubiewski, C.L. Redman, J. Wu, X. Bai, and J.M. Briggs. 2008.
Global change and the ecology of cities. Science 319:756–760.
Healy, W.M., and C.J.E. Welsh. 1992. Evaluating line transects to monitor gray squirrel populations.
Wildlife Society Bulletin 20:83–90.
Hein, E.W. 1997. Demonstration of line transect methodologies to estimate urban gray squirrel density.
Environmental Management 21:943–947.
Hodgkison, S., J.M. Hero, and J. Warnken. 2007. The efficacy of small-scale conservation efforts, as
assessed on Australian golf courses. Biological Conservation 135:576–586.
Homer, C.G., J.A. Dewitz, L. Yang, S. Jin, P. Danielson, G. Xian, J. Coulston, N.D. Herold, J.D.
Wickham, and K. Megown. 2015. Completion of the 2011 national land cover database for the
conterminous United States: Representing a decade of land cover change information. Photogrammetric
Engineering and Remote Sensing 81:345–354.
Hope, R.M. 2013. Rmisc: Rmisc: Ryan Miscellaneous. R package version 1.5. https://CRAN.Rproject.
org/package=Rmisc.
IUCN Invasive Species Specialist Group (IUCN ISSG). 2018. Global Invasive Species Database:
Gray Squirrel. Available online at http://www.iucngisd.org/gisd/species.php?sc=65. Accessed 21
March 2019.
Johnson, M.T.J., and J. Munshi-South. 2017. Evolution of life in urban environments. Science
358:eaam8327.
Koprowski, J.L. 1994. Sciurus carolinensis. Mammalian Species 480:1–9.
Kuhn, M. 2019. caret: Classification and Regression Training. R package version 6.0-84. https://
CRAN.R-project.org/package=caret.
Lapiedra, O., Z. Chejanovski, and J.J. Kolbe. 2016. Urbanization and biological invasion shape animal
personalities. Global Change Biology 23:592–603.
Lenth, R. 2017. Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means (R package emmeans version
1.3.3). Available online at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.
Lima, S.L., and L.M. Dill. 1990. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and
prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:619–640.
Lowry, H., A. Lill, and B.B.M. Wong. 2013. Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments.
Biological Reviews 88:537–549.
Lyons, J., G. Mastromonaco, D.B. Edwards, and A.I. Schulte-Hostedde. 2017. Fat and happy in the
city: Eastern chipmunks in urban environments. Behavioral Ecolo gy 28:1464–1471.
Manski, D.A., L.W. Van Druff, and V. Flyger. 1980. Activities of gray squirrels and people in a downtown
Washington, D.C. park: Management implications. Transactions of the North American
Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference 46:439–454.
McKinney, M.L. 2008. Effects of urbanization on species richness: A review of plants and animals.
Urban Ecosystems 11:161–176.
Nixon, C.M., and L.P. Hansen. 1987. Managing forests to maintain populations of gray and fox squirrels.
Illinois Department of Conservation Technical Bulletin 5:1–35.
NOAA. 2019. Hourly Weather Observations from NOAA’s National Weather Service. Available online
at https://www.weather.gov/lsx/hourly. Accessed on 11 February 2019.
Parker, T.S., and C.H. Nilon. 2008. Gray squirrel density, habitat suitability, and behavior in urban
parks. Urban Ecosystems 11:243–255.
Parker, T.S., and C.H. Nilon. 2012. Urban landscape characteristics correlated with the synurbization
of wildlife. Landscape and Urban Planning 106:316–325.
Powell, L.J.H., Jr. 1982. The occurrence and distribution of selected mammalian species and their
interaction with residents in an urban area. Thesis. State University of New York, Syracuse, NY,
USA. 113 pp.
R Core Team. 2019. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical
Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available online at https://www.R-project.org/.
Samia, D.S., S. Nakagawa, F. Nomura, T.F. Rangel, and D.T. Blumstein. 2015. Increased tolerance to
humans among disturbed wildlife. Nature Communications 6:8877.
Urban Naturalist
L.D. Engel, E.J. Carlen, J.B. Losos, and K.M. Winchell
2020 No. 33
16
Sparkman, A., S. Howe, S. Hynes, B. Hobbs, and K. Handal. 2018. Parallel behavioral and morphological
divergence in fence lizards on two college campuses. Plos One 13:e01 91800.
Stankowich, T., and D.T. Blumstein. 2005. Fear in animals: A meta-analysis and review of risk assessment.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 2 72:2627–2634.
Thompson, D.C. 1977. Diurnal and seasonal activity of the grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Canadian
Journal of Zoology 55:1185–1189.
Thompson, D. 1978. The social system of the grey squirrel. Behaviour 64:305–328.
Tounzen, M.R., D. Epperson, and J.F. Taulman. 2012. Home range and habitat selection of Eastern
gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) in a small urban hardwood forest. Transactions of the Kansas
Academy of Science 115:89–101.
Tratalos, J., R.A. Fuller, K.L. Evans, R.G. Davies, S.E. Newson, J.J.D. Greenwood, and K.J. Gaston.
2007. Bird densities are associated with household densities. Global Change Biology 13:1685–
1695.
Uchida, K., K. Suzuki, T. Shimamoto, H. Yanagawa, and I. Koizumi. 2016. Seasonal variation of
flight initiation distance in Eurasian red squirrels in urban versus rural habitat. Journal of Zoology
298:225–231.
Uhlig, H. 1957. Gray Squirrel populations in extensive forested areas of West Virginia. The Journal
of Wildlife Management 21:335–341.
U.S. Census Bureau. 2017. TIGER/Line Shapefile, county, St. Louis city, MO, All Roads County-based
Shapefile. Available online at https://catalog.data.gov/dataset/tiger-line-shapefile-2017-county-stlouis-
city-mo-all-roads-county-based-shapefile. Accessed 1 March 2019.
U.S. Census Bureau, Population Division. 2019. Annual Estimates of the Resident Population: April
1, 2010 to July 1, 2018. Available online at https://factfinder.census.gov/. Accessed 10 July 2019.
Venables, W.N. and Ripley, B.D. 2002. Modern Applied Statistics with S. Fourth Edition. Springer,
New York, NY. 498 pp.
Wickham, H. 2007. Reshaping Data with the reshape Package. Journal of Statistical Software, 21:1–
20. Available online at http://www.jstatsoft.org/v21/i12/.
Wickham, H. 2016. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag, New York. Available
online at https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org.
Wickham, H., R. François, L. Henry, and K. Müller. 2019. dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation.
R package version 0.8.0.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr
Williamson, R.D. 1983. Identification of urban habitat components which affect eastern gray squirrel
abundance. Urban Ecology 7:345–356.
Winchell, K.M. and J.P. Gibbs. 2016. Golf courses as habitat for aquatic turtles in urbanized landscapes.
Landscape and Urban Planning 147:59–70.
Winchell, K.M., A.C. Battles, and T.Y. Moore. 2020. Locomotor evolution in urban environments. In
M. Szulkin, J. Munshi-South, and A. Charmantier (Eds.). Urban Evolutionary Biology. Oxford
University Press, Oxford, UK. 352 pp.
Ydenberg, R., and L. Dill. 1986. The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of
Behavior 16:229–249.