2012 NORTHEASTERN NATURALIST 19(4):685–690
Effect of Nestling Sex Ratio on the Provisioning Behavior
of Sialia sialis (Eastern Bluebird)
Barbara E. Kieffer1 and Gary Ritchison1,*
Abstract - To maximize reproductive success, parents may differentially invest in sons
and daughters, i.e., sex-biased parental investment. Preferential provisioning behavior has
been reported in one population of Sialia sialis (Eastern Bluebird) and attributed to local
resource competition. We studied the provisioning behavior of Eastern Bluebirds in Madison
County, KY in 2004. We experimentally manipulated brood sex ratios in 24 bluebird
nests, creating female-biased (n = 8), male-biased (n = 5), and control (n = 11) nests. Following
manipulation, nests were videotaped to record adult provisioning behavior. Brood
sex ratio had no effect on the provisioning behavior of either male or female Eastern Bluebirds.
Similarly, we found no effect of brood sex ratio on the provisioning rates of either
male or female bluebirds for control broods that naturally differed in number of male and
female nestlings (n = 9; 5 male-biased and 4 female-biased). Our results contrast with those
of a previous study that found that male Eastern Bluebirds fed nestling males less than nestling
females, possibly because sons were more likely to compete with them for resources in
the future. However, local resource competition seems unlikely because Eastern Bluebirds
exhibit low rates of philopatry. Given these conflicting results, additional study is needed
to determine if factors such as latitude, food availability, or availability of other resources
might influence the provisioning behavior of male Eastern Bluebirds.
Introduction
Sex-biased parental investment can potentially occur via sex-biased provisioning
of nestlings and might be expected if male and female young differ in their
energetic needs. For example, males are typically larger than females in sexually
size-dimorphic species, and male offspring with greater energetic needs may be
fed at higher rates than females (e.g., Green 2002, Magrath et al. 2007).
Differential provisioning might occur in species of birds with little or no
sexual size dimorphism if the sexes differ in competitive abilities or physiological
requirements (e.g., Boncoraglio et al. 2008). In addition, parents might
differentially provision different-sexed young if male and female offspring differentially
affect the reproductive success of parents after independence (Michler
et al. 2010). For example, because male birds tend to be more philopatric than
females (Greenwood 1980), males might differentially provision female nestlings
because, after fledging, philopatric male offspring might represent potential
competitors for important resources (Harper 1985, Stamps 1990). Alternatively,
parents might differentially provision female offspring because females tend to
disperse greater distances, and extra food during development might improve
their chances of survival during and after dispersal (Stamps 1990).
1Department of Biological Sciences, Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY
40475. *Corresponding author - gary.ritchison@eku.edu.
686 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 19, No. 4
Most studies to date have reported little evidence of sex-biased provisioning in
species of birds with little or no sexual size dimorphism (Lee et al. 2010, Leonard
et al. 1994, Michler et al. 2010). However, Gowaty and Droge (1991) reported
sex-biased provisioning by Sialia sialis L. (Eastern Bluebird) in South Carolina,
with males provisioning female-biased broods at higher rates than male-biased
broods and selectively provisioning females within broods. Differences in energetic
needs do not explain this behavior because the metabolic rates of young
male and female bluebirds are similar (Droge et al. 1991). Rather, Gowaty and
Droge (1991) suggested that male Eastern Bluebirds fed female nestlings more
frequently than male nestlings because males are more philopatric and, therefore,
more likely to compete with the same-sexed parent for resources such as mates,
territories, or food resources (i.e., local resource competition).
Because sex-biased provisioning has been reported in so few species and there
is little evidence for local resource competition between adult birds and their
offspring (Weatherhead and Montgomerie 1995), additional study is needed to
determine if birds exhibit sex-biased provisioning and, specifically, to determine
if male Eastern Bluebirds in other populations exhibit a sex-bias in their provisioning
behavior. Our objective was to determine if the provisioning behavior of
male and female Eastern Bluebirds in Kentucky is influenced by brood sex ratio.
Methods
We studied Eastern Bluebirds at the Blue Grass Army Depot (BGAD; 37°41'N,
084°13'W), located southeast of Richmond, Madison County, KY, from 10 May
to 20 July 2004. The BGAD encompasses 5865 ha and consists of grasslands,
open fields, pastures, and scattered woodlots.
Prior to the breeding season, nest boxes mounted on 1.5-m poles were placed
throughout the BGAD. Beginning in May 2004, boxes were checked every 7 to 10
days to determine if boxes were being used by bluebirds. If so, nests were checked
every 3 to 6 days to determine laying dates, hatching dates, and nestling age.
Adult bluebirds were captured and uniquely banded with a numbered USGS
aluminum color banded plus a unique combination of 3 colored plastic leg bands.
Adults were captured in mist nets, either by luring adults into nets by playback
of bluebird songs or nestling distress calls or by placing nets in front of nest-box
entrances. All adults were captured a minimum of two days before provisioning
rates were determined (see below).
We determined the sex of nestling bluebirds by plumage coloration (Gowaty
and Plissner 1998, Pinkowski 1974). Emerging primary and tail feathers of male
nestlings are bright blue, whereas those of females are dull, gray-black with a
faint blue hue. Primary and tail feathers are sufficiently emerged from sheaths
to allow sex determination by 11–13 days post-hatching (B.E. Kieffer and
G. Ritchison, pers. observ.; Gowaty and Plissner 1998; Pyle 1987 ).
We performed manipulations at 24 bluebird nests, with nests placed in one of
three categories: control, female-biased, and male-biased. Manipulation involved
changing the sex ratios of nests from the original ratio to either male- or female2012
B.E. Kieffer and G. Ritchison 687
biased (Lessells et al. 1998). Control nests were subjected to the same procedure
as manipulated nests, but nestlings were switched between boxes without changing
sex ratios.
Manipulations occurred when nestlings were old enough to be sexed, but sufficiently
young so they would not fledge prematurely due to handling (Droge et
al. 1991). Nestlings were exchanged between nests with similar brood sizes (± 1
nestling) and nestlings of similar age (± 1 day). For example, if nest box A had
three female and two male nestlings and nest box B had two female and two male
nestlings similar in age, then two males from box A would be moved to box B and
two females from B would be moved to box A. This manipulation would create
one female-biased box (box A) and one male-biased (box B). Control boxes were
subjected to the same procedure, but equal numbers of nestlings of the same sex
were exchanged.
Nests were videotaped to record adult provisioning behavior. A plastic container
(64 cm x 36 cm x 34 cm high; hereafter camcorder box) mounted on poles
was attached to the back of nest boxes 1–3 days before videotaping began to
acclimate the birds to its presence. A black cardboard box, comparable in size
to the camcorder, was placed in camera boxes to simulate the presence of a camcorder.
Backs of nest boxes were removed when camcorder boxes were attached
and were replaced with wire mesh to prevent adults and nestlings from entering
camcorder boxes. Manipulation of boxes and broods had no apparent effect on
adult behavior; provisioning rates of bluebirds in our study were similar to those
reported previously (Gowaty and Plissner 1998, Pitts 1976).
When videotaping, camcorders were placed in the camcorder boxes and focused
on the inside of nest boxes. Nests were videotaped for 2 to 4 hours daily,
with all taping during the period from sunrise to 12:00 EDT.
We subsequently reviewed videotapes and, for each nest, determined the
total number of visits by each adult and the total time each nest was videotaped.
We used one-way analysis of variance to examine the possible effects
of nestling age and brood size on the provisioning rates (feedings/hour/
nestling) of male and female Eastern Bluebirds. Possible differences among
treatments and by season (months) in provisioning rates of male and female
bluebirds were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance. All statistical
analyses were performed using the Statistical Analysis System (SAS Institute
2002). Values are presented as means ± SE.
Results
We conducted experiments with 24 pairs of bluebirds, including 11 controls, 8
female-biased broods, and 5 male-biased broods. Mean brood size was 4.0 ± 0.1
(range = 3–5) and the mean age of nestlings when we videotaped nests was 14.2
± 0.3 days post-hatching (range = 12–17 days). Nests were taped for an average
of 6.8 ± 0.5 hrs (range = 2–12 hours). Control, male-biased, and female-biased
nests did not differ in either age of young when videotaped (F2, 21 = 0.5, P = 0.63)
or brood size (F2, 21 = 0.7, P = 0.49).
688 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 19, No. 4
Among experimentally skewed broods (i.e., all male, all female, or control
broods), brood sex ratio had no effect on the provisioning rates of either male
(F2, 15 = 0.4, P = 0.66) or female (F2, 15 = 0.2, P = 0.84) Eastern Bluebirds (Fig. 1).
In addition, provisioning rates of male (F2, 15 = 1.9, P = 0.19) and female (F2, 15 =
2.5, P = 0.15) bluebirds did not vary among months (May, June, and July), and
interactions between treatment and month were not significant (males: P = 0.59;
females: P = 0.67). For control broods that naturally differed in the number of
male and female nestlings (n = 9; 5 male-biased and 4 female-biased), we also
found no effect of brood sex ratio on the provisioning rates of either male (F1, 7 =
0.1, P = 0.72) or female (F1, 7 = 1.7, P = 0.30) bluebirds.
Discussion
We found that brood sex ratios did not affect the provisioning behavior of
male and female Eastern Bluebirds. Similar results have been reported for Parus
major L. (Great Tit) (Michler et al. 2010), Paradoxornis webbianus Gould
(Vinous-throated Parrotbill) (Lee et al. 2010), and Sialia mexicana Swainson
(Western Bluebird) (Leonard et al. 1994). In contrast, Gowaty and Droge (1991)
reported that male Eastern Bluebirds fed female-biased broods at higher rates
than male-biased broods in a population in South Carolina. In general, young
males tend to be more philopatric than females (Greenwood 1980) and, therefore,
may be more likely to compete with male parents for food, nest sites, and other
resources (local resource competition). If so, adult males might be expected to
preferentially provision female nestlings, i.e., the non-competing sex (Koenig
and Dickinson 1996). Gowaty and Droge (1991) suggested the preferential feeding
of female nestlings by male Eastern Bluebirds could be explained by the
possibility of such local resource competition.
Figure 1. Mean (SE) provisioning rates of male and female Eastern Bluebirds with control
broods (brood sex ratio not changed), male-biased broods, and female-biased broods.
2012 B.E. Kieffer and G. Ritchison 689
Leonard et al. (1994), however, suggested that it was not clear for any species
why competition with kin would be worse than competition with non-kin and,
in some situations, competition with kin would seem preferable to competition
with non-kin. For example, losing a portion of a territory to a son that could then
breed would be preferable, in terms of a parent’s fitness, to losing a portion of a
territory to an unrelated male (Leonard et al. 1994). In addition, local resource
competition seems unlikely because birds in general, including male birds, exhibit
low rates of philopatry (Weatherhead and Montgomerie 1995). For Eastern
Bluebirds in South Carolina, Gowaty and Plissner (1998) reported that, of 3798
banded and fledged young, only 0.6% bred at their natal boxes and only 1.7%
bred in territories adjacent to natal territories. In addition, return rates to natal
areas are even lower at higher latitudes (Gowaty and Plissner 1998). Available
evidence, therefore, indicates that, for Eastern Bluebirds and other songbirds,
local resource competition is unlikely to occur and unlikely to influence adult
provisioning behavior.
Provisioning of nestlings by parents may be influenced more by nestling behavior
than parental behavior, particularly in cavity-nesting birds where older
young are fed at the cavity entrance. Leonard et al. (1994) found that nestlings that
were fed by cavity-nesting adult Western Bluebirds were closer to entrance holes
and started begging sooner than their siblings. Similarly, Hofstetter and Ritchison
(1998) found that nestling Megascops asio L. (Eastern Screech-Owl) fed by adults
arriving at cavity entrances started begging earlier and positioned their bills closer
to adults. In these species, and perhaps other cavity-nesting birds including Eastern
Bluebirds, the ability of parents to selectively feed particular offspring or offspring
of a certain sex may be limited, particularly later in the nestling period when parents
may not enter cavities to feed young, but, rather, feeding the nestling closest
to, or actually extending their head or bill out of, the cavity entrance.
Empirical evidence for sex-biased provisioning by free-living birds is limited
to a single study of Eastern Bluebirds (Gowaty and Droge 1991), and the reasons
for differences between the results of other studies, including our results, and
those of Gowaty and Droge (1991) are unclear. Additional studies of the possible
effect of brood sex ratios on the provisioning behavior of Eastern Bluebirds and
other species are needed to determine if factors such as latitude (e.g., variation
in degree of natal philopatry), food availability, or availability of other resources
(e.g., suitable cavities) might influence male behavior.
Acknowledgments
We thank Kayde Gilbert and Jacqueline Bennett for assistance in the field, Susan
Smith and two anonymous reviewers for useful comments, and the US Army for providing
access to our study area.
Literature Cited
Boncoraglio, G., R. Martinelli, and N. Saino. 2008. Sex-related asymmetry in competitive
ability of sexually monomorphic Barn Swallow nestlings. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology 62:729–738.
690 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 19, No. 4
Droge, D.L., P.A. Gowaty, and W.W. Weathers. 1991. Sex-biased provisioning: A test for
differences in field metabolic rates of nestling Eastern Bluebirds. Condor 93:793–798.
Gowaty, P.A., and D.L. Droge. 1991. Sex-ratio conflict and the evolution of sex-biased
provisioning in birds. Pp. 932–945, In N.J. Adams and R.H. Slotow (Eds.). Proceedings
of the 20th International Ornithology Congress, BirdLife South Africa, Johannesburg,
South Africa.
Gowaty, P.A., and J.H. Plissner. 1998. Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis). In A. Poole (Ed.)
The Birds of North America Online. Cornell Lab of Ornithology. Ithaca, NY.
Green, D.J. 2002. Pair-bond duration influences paternal provisioning and the primary
sex ratio of Brown Thornbill broods. Animal Behaviour 64:791–800.
Greenwood, P. 1980. Mating systems, philopatry, and dispersal in birds and mammals.
Animal Behaviour 28:1140–1162.
Harper, D.G.C. 1985. Brood division in robins. Animal Behaviour 33:466–480.
Hofstetter, S.H., and G. Ritchison. 1998. The begging behavior of nestling Eastern
Screech-Owls. Wilson Bulletin 110:86–92.
Koenig, W.D., and J.L. Dickinson. 1996. Nestling sex-ratio variation in Western Bluebirds.
Auk 113:902–910.
Lee, J.-W., H.-Y. Kim, and B.J. Hatchwell. 2010. Parental provisioning behaviour in a
flock-living passerine, the Vinous-throated Parrotbill, Paradoxornis webbianus. Journal
of Ornithology 151:483–490.
Leonard, M.L., K.L. Teather, A. G. Horn, W.D. Koenig, and J.L. Dickinson. 1994. Provisioning
in Western Bluebirds is not related to offspring sex. Behavioral Ecology
5:455–459.
Lessells, C.M., K.R. Oddiet, and A.C. Mateman. 1998. Parental behaviour is unrelated to
experimentally manipulated Great Tit brood sex ratio. Animal Behaviour 56:385–393.
Magrath, M.J.L., E. Van Lieshout, I. Pen, G.H. Visser, and J. Komdeur. 2007. Estimating
expenditure on male and female offspring in a sexually size-dimorphic bird: A comparison
of different methods. Journal of Animal Ecology 76:1169–1180.
Michler, S.P.M., M. Bleeker, M. van der Velde, C. Both, J. Komdeur, and J.M. Tinbergen.
2010. Parental provisioning in relation to offspring sex and sex ratio in the Great Tit
(Parus major). Behaviour 147:1355–1378.
Pinkowski, B.C. 1974. A comparative study of the behavioral and breeding ecology
of the Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis). Ph.D. Dissertation. Wayne State University,
Detroit, MI.
Pitts, T.D. 1976. Nesting habits of Eastern Bluebirds in northwest Tennessee. Ph.D. Dissertation.
University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.
Pyle, P. 1987. Identification Guide to North American Birds. Part 1. Slate Creek Press,
Bolins, CA.
SAS Institute. 2002. SAS 9.1 for Windows. SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC.
Stamps, J.A. 1990. When should avian parents differentially provision sons and daughters?
American Naturalist 135:671–685.
Weatherhead, P.J., and R. Montgomerie. 1995. Local resource competition and sex ratio
variation in birds. Journal of Avian Biology 26:168–171.