Overwater Movement of Raccoons (Procyon lotor) in a Naturally Fragmented Coastal Landscape
Raymond D. Dueser, Nancy D. Moncrief, Oskars Keišs, Joel D. Martin, John H. Porter, and Barry R. Truitt
Northeastern Naturalist, Volume 20, Issue 3 (2013): 511–528
Full-text pdf (Accessible only to subscribers. To subscribe click here.)
Access Journal Content
Open access browsing of table of contents and abstract pages. Full text pdfs available for download for subscribers.
Current Issue: Vol. 30 (3)
Check out NENA's latest Monograph:
Monograph 22
511
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
22001133 NORNToHrEthAeSaTstEeRrnN N NaAtuTrUaRlisAtLIST 2V0(o3l). :2501,1 N–5o2. 83
Overwater Movement of Raccoons (Procyon lotor) in a
Naturally Fragmented Coastal Landscape
Raymond D. Dueser1,2, Nancy D. Moncrief 2,*, Oskars Keišs1, Joel D. Martin1,
John H. Porter 3, and Barry R. Truitt4
Abstract - Procyon lotor (Raccoon) is a major predator of beach-nesting and colonial
waterbirds on the Virginia barrier islands. An understanding of water as a barrier
to inter-island movement by Raccoons will be essential to effective management of
these predators in this naturally fragmented coastal environment. We examined 4 independent
lines of direct evidence for Raccoon movement between 1999 and 2007:
1) locations of recaptured, ear-tagged Raccoons on both the islands and the adjacent
mainland, 2) overland movements of radio-collared Raccoons, 3) inter-island
movements of radio-collared resident Raccoons, and 4) movements of translocated
Raccoons. We recaptured 78 of 177 ear-tagged island Raccoons, all on the same island
as the initial capture. We also tagged and released 65 mainland Raccoons, none
of which was ever recaptured on an island. We often observed overland movements
>1 km per day by radio-collared animals on both the islands and the mainland. Nevertheless,
only 3 of 51 (6%) collared animals (2 males and 1 female) moved overwater
from the location where they were captured. None of the 4 Raccoons radio-collared
on the mainland moved to an island. Although Raccoons in this system are highly
mobile, overwater movements seem to be infrequent events; only 3 of 234 tagged/
collared island individuals moved between islands, and none of the 69 tagged/collared
mainland individuals moved to an island. Finally, we observed return movements
by 22 of the 32 (69%) animals (11 males and 11 females) that were translocated either
from the mainland to a nearby island or between adjacent islands. Translocated
animals exhibited a much greater tendency than resident animals to make overwater
crossings. In all cases of overwater movement, the water channels were relatively
shallow and relatively slow moving. None of the 335 marked animals in this study
crossed a tidal inlet. The mobility observed here is consistent with the idea that the
distribution of Raccoons on the islands has expanded in recent decades. Predation
management on these islands will require a strategic approach that takes into account
both island isolation and Raccoon mobility.
Introduction
Mammalian predators, including Procyon lotor L. (Raccoon), have caused the
decline and/or extinction of countless populations of island-nesting waterbirds
and seabirds (Burger and Gochfeld 1994). The avifaunas of entire archipelagos
have been altered dramatically by the introduction of mammalian predators (e.g.,
1Department of Wildland Resources, Utah State University, Logan UT 84322. 2Virginia
Museum of Natural History, Martinsville, VA 24112. 3Department of Environmental Sciences,
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904. 4Virginia Coast Reserve, The
Nature Conservancy, Nassawadox, VA 23413. *Corresponding author - nancy.moncrief@
vmnh.virginia.gov.
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
512
Bailey 1993). The Virginia barrier islands support a diverse assemblage of beachnesting
and colonial waterbirds (Williams et al. 2007). Sandy beaches, overwash
fans, dunes, and shrub thickets provide extensive habitat for 27 species of herons,
egrets, ibises, pelicans, gulls, terns, oystercatchers, skimmers, and plovers. Most
of these birds are ground-nesters, and thus are highly vulnerable to mammalian
predation. Charadrius melodus Ord (Piping Plover) and C. wilsonia Ord (Wilson’s
Plover) are both state-endangered species in Virginia; the Piping Plover is
a federally threatened species (Terwilliger 1991). The Conservation Action Plan
for the Avian Communities in the Virginia Barrier Island System (Barrier Island
Avian Partnership 1996) identified mammalian predators as one of the primary
continuing threats to the success of avian conservation on the islands.
Numerous studies cite predation by Raccoons as a major cause for the precipitous
decline in numbers of beach-nesting and colonial waterbirds on the Virginia
barrier islands during the past 50 years (Boettcher et al. 2007; Brinker et al. 2007;
Wilke et al. 2007; Williams et al. 1990, 2005, 2007). Erwin et al. (2001) proposed
that the distribution of the Raccoon on these islands has expanded during this
time, thus exposing more nesting habitat and more avian colonies to the effects
of predation. Most islands are owned and managed to provide nesting habitat
for shorebirds and colonial waterbirds by The Nature Conservancy (TNC), the
US Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS), the Virginia Department of Game and
Inland Fisheries (VDGIF), and the Virginia Department of Conservation and
Recreation (VDCR). In an effort to reduce predation pressure on nesting birds,
TNC, USFWS, VDGIF, and US Department of Agriculture Wildlife Services
(WS) have instituted an extensive program to remove Raccoons from the Virginia
barrier islands. Predation management has become an ongoing part of conservation
activity on these islands.
Effective management of Raccoons and other meso-predators requires an understanding
of animal movement across the landscape (Martin et al. 2010, Roth
et al. 2008, Waldstein 2010). In particular, management of island populations
requires information about the role of water as a barrier to movement. Previous
studies provide conflicting information about the propensity of Raccoons to cross
water channels. Gehrt (2003) reported that Raccoons can cross water easily and
that they probably move overwater frequently in some areas. In contrast, Kauhala
(1996) and Rosatte et al. (2010) reported that water can be a barrier to movement
by Raccoons. Although many researchers have studied movement of Raccoons in
eastern North America (Gehrt 2003)—in part, a result of the growing interest in
control of disease transmission by this species (Rosatte et al. 2009, 2010)—only
a few studies have reported information about island populations on the Atlantic
coast (Waldstein 2010). Moreover, these studies did not focus on movement between
islands.
Given the contradictory evidence about overwater movement outlined above,
and the paucity of information about movement in coastal island populations,
our objective was to obtain gender-specific estimates of the timing, frequency,
and trajectory of movements by Raccoons in this coastal system. Specifically,
513
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
we sought to determine 1) how frequently Raccoons move between the mainland
and islands or between islands, 2) the influence of island isolation, measured as
the width of a water gap, on the probability of movement, 3) whether males and
females are equally likely to move, 4) whether movements vary seasonally, and
5) whether there is a tendency for animals to move from islands of low habitat
quality to islands with higher quality habitat. Based on our observations, we assessed
the likelihood that the distribution of Raccoons has recently expanded on
the islands.
Study Area
Study sites included 9 locations on the mainland of the southern Delmarva
Peninsula and 12 of the barrier islands that extend ≈150 km along the seaward
margin of the Peninsula. The islands are centered at approximately 37º30'N
and 75º40'W in Accomack and Northampton counties, VA (Fig. 1). This 1000-
km2 landscape is a dynamic, highly fragmented mosaic of open bays, marshes,
marsh islands, back barriers, and barrier islands (Hayden et al. 1991). The islands
are located 0.4–12.1 km offshore, range from 1 to 10 m in elevation, and
vary from 27–7029 ha in area. Vegetation types are diverse and include emergent
sandbars, low-lying marsh, grasslands with extensive overwash zones, and
shrub thickets and mature forests on elevated islands (McCaffrey and Dueser
1990). The islands are separated by estuarine marshes and bays that connect
to the Atlantic Ocean through deep inlets (Oertel et al. 1989). As measured
from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) Coastal
Change Analysis Program (C-CAP) land-cover data layers for the lower Delmarva
Peninsula (Virginia and Maryland) for the year 2001 (http://www.csc.
noaa.gov/crs/lca/ccap.html), the average distance between nearest-neighbor islands
was 808 m (SE = 162), and the average distance between adjacent islands
that are separated by deep, swift-running inlets was 518 m (SE = 84). The average
distance from the mainland was 5835 m (SE = 745; range = 351–12,868
m). Given the rates of erosion and accretion in this dynamic environment, the
distances between nearest-neighbor islands might be subject to changes on the
order of tens of meters in any given year.
Several islands have been occupied by humans sporadically since the 1600s
but have been deserted since a series of severe storms in the early 1930s (Badger
and Kellam 1989, Barnes and Truitt 1997, Graham 1976a). Except for a few
small, scattered private in-holdings, the islands are held in public ownership by
the USFWS or the Commonwealth of Virginia or are owned by TNC. TNC holdings
comprise the Virginia Coast Reserve (VCR), a National Science Foundation
(NSF) long-term ecological research (LTER) site, a Man and the Biosphere reserve,
and a Western Hemisphere International Shorebird Reserve Network site
(Badger 1978, 1991, 1997).
At least 11 islands, including Assateague, Cedar, Chincoteague, Fishermans,
Hog, Mockhorn, Parramore, Revel, Skidmore, Smith, and Wallops, support potential
source populations of Raccoons (Fig. 1; Keišs 2001). Raccoons also occur
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
514
occasionally on at least 13 other islands, including Assawoman, Chimney Pole,
Cobb, Fowling Point, Holly Bluff, Little Cobb, Metompkin, Mink, Myrtle, Raccoon,
Rogue, Ship Shoal, and Wreck (Fig. 1). This study focuses on the islands
Figure 1. Location of islands and marshes of the Virginia barrier island complex and
selected sites on the adjacent mainland.
515
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
from Parramore southward to Fishermans. Fishermans is connected to the southern
tip of the Delmarva Peninsula by a highway bridge, but all the other islands
in this study are accessible only by boat. Additionally, tidal conditions restrict the
timing and duration of access to these islands. The study islands include marsh
(Fowling Point, Mink, Swash), grassland (Myrtle, Ship Shoal), shrubland (Hog,
Holly Bluff, Rogue) and forested (Fishermans, Parramore, Revel, Skidmore,
Smith) habitats. Given the effects of area and elevation, habitat complexity on
these islands is cumulative, so that forested islands also have areas of shrubland,
grassland and marsh; shrubland islands have grassland and marsh; and grassland
islands have marsh (Dueser and Brown 1980).
Methods
We obtained and examined 4 independent lines of direct evidence about
movements of Raccoons on Virginia barrier islands and the adjacent mainland between
June 1999 and December 2007: 1) locations of recaptured Raccoons based
on mark-release-recapture sampling, 2) overland movements of radio-collared
Raccoons presumed to be resident on specific islands or on the mainland, 3)
inter-island movements of radio-collared Raccoons, and 4) movements of translocated
Raccoons. Each data set included males and females, mainland and island
animals, and animals on different islands, and each involved long-term monitoring.
The use of multiple, independent data sets provided diverse opportunities,
circumstances, and time spans for observing overwater movement.
Mark-release-recapture
Raccoons were trapped, tagged, and released using the methods of Keišs
(2001) and Martin (2007). Large single-door cage traps (90 x 30 x 25 cm, Tomahawk
Live Trap Company, Tomahawk, WI) were baited during summer with
canned cat food, sardines, and maple syrup and during autumn with fruits of
Diospyros virginiana L. (Common Persimmon), apples, fish, and shrimp. Traps
were set during the day and inspected the following morning. To avoid captured
animals becoming overheated, we covered traps with vegetation collected
on-site. Each animal was immobilized by intramuscular injection of ketamine/
acepromazine solution (10 mL of 100 mg/mL ketamine + 1 mL acepromazine),
using 0.1 mL solution per 1 kg of animal. Body mass was estimated subjectively
before immobilization. Age was estimated as subadult or adult using tooth wear
(Grau et al. 1970), body size, and external reproductive attributes. Gender was
determined, and females were palpated for signs of pregnancy or lactation. Each
Raccoon was weighed using a 5-kg spring-balance with 0.05-kg precision, and
a numbered ear-tag (Monel #3, National Band and Tag Company, Newport, KY)
was attached to each ear.
Between June 1999 and July 2006, we ear-tagged and released Raccoons on
9 islands (Fishermans, Hog, Mink, Mockhorn, Myrtle, Parramore, Revel, Skidmore,
and Smith; Fig. 1). We also tagged and released Raccoons at 9 mainland
sites (Capeville, Gargatha, Kiptopeake, Locustville, Machipongo, Nassawadox,
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
516
Oyster, Shadyside, and Trower; Fig. 1). There was the potential for these tagged
animals to be recaptured on the islands during an extensive predation management
program conducted by TNC, USFWS, VDGIF, and WS during 2001–2007.
They trapped (either annually or semi-annually) and euthanized all Raccoons and
Vulpes vulpes L. (Red Foxes) captured on 12 islands, including Assawoman, Cedar,
Fishermans, Metompkin, Mockhorn, Myrtle, Parramore, Revel, Ship Shoal,
Smith, Wallops, and Wreck (Fig. 1).
Radiotelemetry
We also trapped, tagged, radio-collared, and released a separate set of Raccoons
on 9 islands (Fishermans, Hog, Mink, Mockhorn, Myrtle, Parramore,
Rogue, Skidmore, and Smith; Fig. 1) and 2 mainland sites (Capeville and Nassawadox)
between June 1999 and July 2006. Collared animals were tracked
repeatedly from fixed geographic locations with a collapsible, hand-held Yagi
antenna and a Wildlife Materials TRX-1000S radio-receiver. Island animals were
tracked as often as possible, given the constraints of weather and tides. Mainland
animals were tracked periodically for 12 weeks following release. We attempted
to take >3 bearings for each individual on each tracking occasion, to facilitate
triangulation of animal locations. We were able to take >3 bearings on ≈80% of
the tracking occasions, sufficient to identify the island on which an animal was
located. Capture, release, and tracking locations were recorded with a handheld
Garmin 12 Map GPS unit. Bearings were determined with an azimuth model Suunto
precision compass graduated to 1/2°. Animal locations were computed with
program “Locate” (http://www.locateiii.com/index.htm) and mapped on 1999
Landsat 7 (ETM+) imagery of the study area.
The collars were designed to have a provisional line-of-site range of ≈3.0 km
on this relatively flat, low-lying terrain. In reality, however, several tests indicated
that we were usually within 1–2 km of the re-sighted animal. We defined the
maximum distance moved by an individual as the greatest straight-line distance
between any pair of locations ever observed for the animal. These maximum
movement distances were tested for location differences (mainland vs. island),
gender differences (all males vs. all females), and gender differences for island
animals only with nonparametric Mann-Whitney 2-sample rank tests adjusted
for tied ranks (Zar 1999). Maximum movement distances were also tested for
differences among islands (Parramore vs. Hog-Rogue vs. Smith) with a Kruskal-
Wallis single-factor analysis of variance by ranks adjusted for tied ranks (Zar
1999). Although analyses were based on ranks, we report maximum movement
distances as means and standard errors for purposes of comparison.
Translocations
We implemented a translocation study between May 2001 and July 2003 using
another separate set of Raccoons. Individuals were captured from a “source”
area (Nassawadox, Parramore, Revel, Rogue; Fig. 1), translocated to an adjacent
“release” area (Fowling Point, Hog, Parramore, Revel, Swash; Fig. 1), and monitored
frequently by means of radio-telemetry. We captured animals in upland
517
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
habitat on the source area and released them into upland habitat on the release
area. Release areas were adjacent to the source area, free of nesting waterbirds,
and occupied by Raccoons at the time of the study, but separated by a water
channel on all tides. All translocations involved distances greater than the closest
possible distance between source and release areas. Average translocation
distance was ≈5.4 km (range = 1.8–7.4 km), from a point on the upland of the
source area to a point on the upland of the release area.
Our methods followed the 1998 guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists
for the use of mammals in research (Animal Care and Use Committee
1998). All procedures conformed to Utah State University Institutional Animal
Care and Use Committee policies (protocol 952).
Results
Mark-release-recapture
We ear-tagged and released 177 Raccoons on 9 islands and recorded 122 recaptures
(Table 1), ranging from 1 to 7 recaptures per individual (mean = 0.7).
We recaptured 78 individuals (37 males and 41 females) at least once through
December 2007. All recaptures occurred on the same island where the Raccoon
had been tagged and released originally. No individual was observed to move between
islands. The number of days between first and last capture (i.e., total period
of observation for a recaptured individual) ranged from 1 to 1413 d (mean = 307
d, SE = 34). We also ear-tagged and released 65 Raccoons at 9 mainland sites
(Fig. 1): Capeville (2 individuals), Gargatha (9), Kiptopeake (10), Locustville
(9), Machipongo (4), Nassawadox (26), Oyster (2), Shadyside (1), and Trower
(2). Despite the capture of 936 Raccoons on Cedar, Fishermans, Metompkin,
Mockhorn, Myrtle, Parramore, Revel, and Smith islands during extensive removal-
trapping between 2001 and 2007, none of the tagged mainland individuals was
ever recaptured on an island.
Table 1. Recapture locations of 177 ear-tagged Procyon lotor (Raccoons) on 9 Virginia barrier
islands 1999–2007. n = number of individuals tagged and released.
# of individuals recaptured
# of recaptures on same on different Recaptured Recaptured
Island n on same island island island males females
Fishermans 2 0 0 0 0 0
Hog 22 3 3 0 2 1
Mink 1 0 0 0 0 0
Mockhorn 8 2 2 0 1 1
Myrtle 4 0 0 0 0 0
Parramore 92 63 46 0 24 22
Revel 2 2 2 0 1 1
Skidmore 24 43 17 0 6 11
Smith 22 9 8 0 3 5
Total 177 122 78 0 37 41
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
518
Overland movements
To learn about overland movement of Raccoons, we radio-collared and released
30 adult and sub-adult individuals (18 males and 12 females) on 5 islands
and at 2 locations on the adjacent mainland in June 1999 (Table 2). We monitored
the locations of these animals as often as possible through June 2000. During this
period, we used 120-g whip-antenna collars purchased from 2 suppliers: AVM
Instrument Company (AVM) and Wildlife Materials, Inc. (WMI). Many of the 20
AVM collars were no longer detectable after only a brief period of exposure to
this wet, salty environment. On average, an animal wearing an AVM collar was
no longer detectable after 25 d (SE = 5.3), whereas an animal wearing a WMI
collar was detectable for an average of 293 d (SE = 77.0). Either there was mass
movement of animals wearing AVM collars to locations where they could not
be detected, or many of these collars simply failed after only a short time in the
field. The latter interpretation is supported by the observations for 12 animals
on Parramore; none of the 3 males and 3 females collared with AVM collars was
detectable after September 1999, whereas the 4 males and 2 females fitted with
WMI collars were still detectable on the island 9 months later in June 2000.
Given our inability to detect many of the 20 AVM collars shortly after they were
deployed, we actually monitored overland movements of 24 of the 30 collared
Raccoons (15 males and 9 females) between June 1999 and June 2000 (Table 2).
Tracking effort per individual ranged from 6 to 17 d on the islands and from 45
to 50 d on the mainland. Tracking period ranged from 1–321 d following release
(mean = 87 d). We resighted 276 Raccoons on the islands and 194 on the mainland.
None of these animals was observed to move between islands or between the
mainland and an island. Maximum distances moved ranged from 316 to 5550 m.
Table 2. Number of Procyon lotor (Raccoons) successfully radio-tracked on 9 islands and 2 mainland
sites (1999–2006). Ten collars were undetectable after release. The number of Raccoons
radio-collared and released is in parenthesis.
Year
Location 1999 2002 2003 2005 2006 Total
Islands
Fishermans - 0 (2) - - - 0 (2)
Hog 3 (3) - - - - 3 (3)
Mink - 1 (1) - - - 1 (1)
Mockhorn - 4 (4) - - - 4 (4)
Myrtle 0 (2) 0 (1) 1 (1) - - 1 (4)
Parramore 9 (12) - - - - 9 (12)
Rogue 1 (1) - - - - 1 (1)
Skidmore - 1 (1) - 10 (10) 10 (10) 21 (21)
Smith 7 (8) 0 (1) - - - 7 (9)
Mainland sites
Capeville 1 (1) - - - - 1 (1)
Nassawadox 3 (3) - - - - 3 (3)
Total 24 (30) 6 (10) 1 (1) 10 (10) 10 (10) 51 (61)
519
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
Rankings of movement distance did not differ (U = 58.5 < U0.05 (2),20,4 = 66, P =
0.141) overall between males (1236 m, SE = 124) and females (1847 m, SE = 610).
Rankings of movement distance did not differ between mainland (2666 m, SE =
1072) and island animals (1225 m, SE = 173; U = 68 < U0.05(2),15,9 = 101, P = 0.99),
or between island males (mean = 1267 m, SE = 141) and island females (mean =
1147 m, SE = 443; U = 59 < U0.05(2),13,7 = 71, P = 0.303). Ranked movement distances
did differ among islands: 1261 m (SE = 261, n = 7) on Smith, 752 m (SE =
130, n = 4) on Hog and Rogue, and 1051 m (SE = 294, n = 9) on Parramore, but not
significantly so (Hc = 2.675 < Χ2
0.05,3 = 7.815, P < 0.50). None of these distances
were great enough to represent a constraint imposed by the size of the island. The
longest 1-day, straight-line movement for an island animal was 1788 m in 24 hours
by male number 8-7 on Smith. The longest 1-day movement for a mainland animal
was 3044 m in 12 hours by female number 88-89 at Nassawadox. The longest
movement observed overall was by female number 87-86 who traveled 5550 m
south from Capeville toward the southern tip of the Peninsula over a period of 6.5
months, where she was road-killed on 27 February 2000.
Inter-island movements
To study inter-island movements, we radio-collared an additional 31 adult and
sub-adult Raccoons (12 males and 19 females) on 6 islands between July 2002
and June 2006 using only WMI collars (Table 2). These included 11 animals (4
males and 7 females) on 6 islands during July to August 2002 and during August
2003 (Table 2); we monitored 7 of these animals for 3–434 d following release
(mean = 233 d). We collared and monitored 10 additional animals (4 males and
6 females) on Skidmore between May and August 2005 (Table 2). Finally, we
collared and monitored 10 more animals (4 males and 6 females) on Skidmore
between June and August 2006 (Table 2). We monitored all 20 of the Skidmore
animals successfully for the duration of the study period (66 d in 2005 and 58 d
in 2006).
Four of the 31 collars failed within 4 d of release. We thus monitored the
post-release locations of 27 individuals (11 males and 16 females). The most intense
periods of monitoring were 1) May 2002–August 2003 (14 tracking days,
7 individuals, 286 re-sightings), when we were looking for movement along
specific potential inter-island pathways, and 2) May–August 2005 (8 tracking
days, 10 individuals, 64 re-sightings) and May–August 2006 (19 tracking days,
10 individuals, 190 re-sightings), when we were closely monitoring the population
on Skidmore.
Between June 2002 and June 2006, the island location of the average animal
was known for 101 d (SE = 21, range = 1–434 d). We detected only 3 inter-island
movements, all by adult animals (Table 3): female number 236-237 moved from
Mink to Myrtle in July 2002, male number 529-530 moved from Myrtle to Mink
in August 2003, and male number 95001 moved from Skidmore to Holly Bluff,
and then to the mainland in July 2005. These movements occurred approximately
2, 3, and 66 d post-release, respectively. The minimum overwater distances
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
520
involved in these movements were 0.5 km (Mink–Myrtle), 0.4 km (Skidmore–
Holly Bluff), and 0.2 km (Holly Bluff–mainland). No mainland-island or interisland
movement was detected for the other 24 radio-collared animals between
June 2002 and June 2006.
Translocations
To further investigate overwater movement, we translocated 32 Raccoons (16
males and 16 females) between 7 areas in 2001–2003 (Table 4). All translocations
were conducted during the warm season, (May–August). We monitored
the post-release movements for 1–385 d (mean = 190 d). No signal was ever
detected for 2 adult males that were translocated from Parramore (1 to Revel and
1 to Swash), and 1 adult male died within 3 d of being moved from Parramore
to Swash. Eight animals (3 males and 5 females) remained on the release area
for 2–259 d (mean = 170 d), and 21 animals left the release area (Table 4). Nineteen
island animals (9 males and 10 females) returned to the source area within
Table 4. Inter-island and island-mainland movements of 32 Procyon lotor (Raccoons) translocated
2001–2003. No post-release signal was received for 1 animal translocated from Parramore to Revel
or for 1 animal translocated from Parramore to Swash. Source location is the site of initial capture.
n = number of individuals collared and translocated. # that stayed = the number of individuals that
stayed at the release location, and # that returned = the number of individuals that moved back to
the source location.
Source Release # that # that # moved to
location n location stayed returned a third location
Parramore 9 Revel 3 5 0
Parramore 6 Swash 0 4 1
Revel 11 Parramore 4 7 0
Revel 3 Swash 1 2 0
Rogue 1 Hog 0 1 0
Nassawadox 2 Fowling Point 0 2 0
Total 32 8 21 1
Table 3. Inter-island movements of 31 radio-collared Procyon lotor (Raccoons) captured and
released on 6 Virginia barrier islands, 2002–2006. Four collars on 3 islands (Fishermans, n = 2;
Myrtle, n = 1; and Smith, n = 1) were no longer detectable within 4 days of release. n = number of
individuals collared and released. # that moved = number of individuals that moved to a different
island.
Island n # that remained # that moved % that moved
Fishermans 2 uncertain uncertain uncertain
Mink 1 0 1 100
Mockhorn 4 4 0 0
Myrtle 2 uncertain 1 50
Skidmore 21 20 1 5
Smith 1 uncertain uncertain uncertain
Total 31 24 3 10
521
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
1–221 d (mean = 39 d). Six of these individuals returned within 1–2 d and 15
within <15 d. The animals that returned were still present on the source area from
5–385 d (mean = 164 d) days following their return.
Island female number 189-188 moved from Swash to Revel rather than
back to Parramore. Both mainland males, numbers 163-164 and 173-172, returned
from Fowling Point to the mainland within 5 d, where they stayed for
the next year. Animal number 163-164 made at least 1 return visit to Fowling
Point during this period. Altogether then, at least 70% of the translocated
animals for which post-release observations are available either returned to the
source location or moved to a third location following release. Minimum upland-
to-upland distances involved in these returns were 0.5 km (Parramore to
Revel), 2.4 km (Parramore to Swash), 3.5 km (Revel to Swash), 0.7 km (Rogue
to Hog), and 0.9 km (Nassawadox to Fowling Point).
Discussion
Raccoons in our study were capable of crossing at least 3 km of open water and
marsh to reach an island. Translocated animals routinely made overwater forays
of 1 km or more to return to their presumed home island. Nevertheless, movement
between islands was relatively rare for resident (non-translocated) animals, even
during the warm season of the year.
Both island and mainland Raccoons made extensive overland movements.
The observed maximum distances moved by island males (2.1 km) and females
(3.5 km) suggest that the extent of movement typically was not constrained by
island size. Several mainland animals moved distances that would have been
long enough to reach several of the islands, if the trajectory of those movements
had been across open water. Overall average movements of males (1.3
km) and females (1.8 km) are comparable to average overland movements
(mean = 1.54 km) reported by Rosatte et al. (2010) for Raccoons in southeastern
ON, Canada.
None of the distances moved by mainland Raccoons would have been long
enough for an animal to disperse directly from the mainland to a remote island
(e.g., Parramore or Hog). Nonetheless, several of the distances would have been
long enough to move from the mainland to a nearby island over open water (e.g.,
Mockhorn or Skidmore). Considering only the observed mobility and the absolute
distances involved, 13 islands in this system appear to be within overwater
dispersal range (≈3 km) of mainland Raccoons. In contrast, 20 islands appear to
be most accessible by movement between islands.
Our data indicate that Raccoons have the ability to move >2 km overland on
the islands, but resident animals seldom moved between islands. We observed
no inter-island movement for 177 (0%) ear-tagged animals (Table 1). This observation
is consistent with evidence from other studies (Rosatte et al. 2007,
2010) indicating that, although Raccoons are capable of overland movements of
more than 20 km, water crossings are relatively rare events. Rosatte et al. (2007)
reported that only 3 of 579 animals (0.5%) crossed the St. Lawrence River.
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
522
Additionally, only 3 of 51 collared residents in our study moved overwater, and
all such movements were less than 1 km straight-line distance. In another study of Raccoons
on the Virginia barrier islands, Hanlon et al. (1989) observed that 14 of 15
collared Raccoons (6 males and 9 females) on Parramore remained on the island
throughout the 9-month study; 1 male moved from Parramore to Revel. Altogether
then, between the present study and the study by Hanlon et al. (1989), only
4 of 66 (6%) collared Raccoons have been observed to move between islands in
this system, and each recorded movement measured less than 1 km. Three of these individuals
were males and 1 was a female.
In contrast to movements of collared residents, 19 of the 27 translocated island
Raccoons for which post-release observations were available either returned
from the release island to the source island or moved to a third island, moving
overwater distances up to 3.6 km. Similarly, the 2 mainland males translocated
to Fowling Point returned to the mainland source area, an overwater distance of
at least 0.9 km. Hanlon et al. (1989) also observed that 3 translocated island Raccoons
(2 males and 1 female) returned to their source island, covering an overwater
distance of at least 0.8 km. Combining the results of the present study and
Hanlon et al. (1989), 24 of 32 (75%) translocated Raccoons, including 13 males
and 11 females, were observed to move across open water, over distances up to
3.6 km. Both males and females exhibited a tendency to move across water to
return to their home island following translocation. Thus, overwater distances of
1–3 km appear to present no challenge to the movement of motivated Raccoons
on the Virginia barrier islands.
Previous studies provide conflicting information on the propensity of Raccoons
to cross water channels. Kauhala (1996) stated that fresh water bodies
only a few hundred meters in width are sufficient to impede Racc oon movement
in Europe. Additionally, Rosatte et al. (2010) reported that activities to control
the spread of rabies in ON, Canada are designed using rivers as partial barriers
to restrict Raccoon movement coming from New York State. On the other hand,
Gehrt (2003) conducted an extensive literature search and concluded that Raccoons
can cross water easily and that they probably move overwater frequently
in some areas. This conclusion is supported by several studies reporting that Raccoons
crossed freshwater barriers 300 m–1 km in width (Arjo et al. 2007, Gehrt
et al. 1993, Kaufmann 1990, Rosatte et al. 2007) and saltwater barriers between
645 m–950 m in width (Bigler et al. 1981, Hartman and Eastman 1999).
Thus, based on the preponderance of evidence (Arjo et al. 2007, Bigler et al.
1981, Gehrt 2003, Gehrt et al. 1993, Hanlon et al. 1989, Hartman and Eastman
1999, Kaufmann 1990, Rosatte et al. 2007), we expected to observe frequent
overwater movement between adjacent islands in the Virginia barrier island complex.
Our observations contradict this expectation. We observed no inter-island
movements by ear-tagged animals, and no mainland animals were recaptured on
the islands. We observed overwater transits by only 3 of 51 (6%) radio-collared
animals. We observed no movement of resident animals from Hog, Mockhorn,
Parramore, Rogue, or Smith, either to another island or to the mainland.
523
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
However, we observed inter-island movements by 19 of 27 (70%) translocated
island animals. Translocated Raccoons readily crossed expanses of open water
and marsh up to 3.4 km in width to return to their home island.
Our four independent data sets indicate that Raccoons are capable of moving
overwater distances equal to the distances between adjacent islands, but they
exhibited little inclination to do so unless motivated by displacement from their
supposed home island. Among the many possible explanations for these observations,
we have data related to two. Movement of individuals from an island might
be driven by high Raccoon abundance on that island. Keišs (2001) trapped Raccoons
on 8 islands in 1999 and 2000; the highest observed capture rates were on
Parramore and Smith, from which no movements were observed over the 8 years
of our study. Furthermore, Hanlon et al. (1989) reported an extraordinary Raccoon
density of ≈37 Raccoons per square kilometer on Parramore, but only 1 case
of inter-island movement by a resident (to Revel). Conversely, the island with
one of the lowest capture rates reported by Keišs (2001) was Myrtle, from which
1 animal was observed to move in our study. Based on this limited evidence, there
is no obvious direct connection between abundance and inclination for Raccoons
to move between islands in this system.
Movement might also be driven by habitat conditions on an island. Keišs
(2001) found trapping success to be positively correlated with area of contiguous
saltmarsh, area of shrubs and forest, and total island area. Saltmarsh habitat
provides a year-round food supply for Raccoons (Waldstein 2010), woody habitat
provides year-round shelter (Gehrt 2003), and larger, higher islands with forest
habitat provide a measure of protection from storms and overwash (Hayden et al.
1991). Only 1 of the 3 animals that we observed to move between islands left an
island of presumably lower habitat quality for a location of higher habitat quality.
Mink is a marsh island, Myrtle a grassland island, and Skidmore a forested
island; all 3 are relatively small (<42 ha). A Mink animal moved to Myrtle and
a Myrtle animal moved to Mink; neither left an island of high habitat quality,
or moved to an island of higher apparent habitat quality. A Skidmore animal
moved from a forested island of apparently high habitat quality to Holly Bluff
(a shrubland island), and then to comparable forest on the mainland. We believe
that shelter is a limiting factor for Raccoon survival—overwinter survival, in
particular—in the rigorous environment on these islands. Nevertheless, based on
these few cases there is no clear evidence that the trajectory of Raccoon movement
is from islands of lower habitat quality to islands of higher habitat quality.
The water barriers that the 3 radio-collared residents traversed (between Mink
and Myrtle, between Skidmore and Holly Bluff, and between Holly Bluff and
the mainland) are relatively shallow and relatively slow moving. It is noteworthy
that the translocated animals crossed similar water channels to return to the
sites where they were captured. This type of water barrier contrasts dramatically
with the deep, fast-flowing tidal inlets that separate the barrier islands (including
Parramore, Hog, Myrtle, and Smith) from each other (Oertel et al. 1989). We
observed no instance of movement across a tidal inlet by any of the 335 marked
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
524
animals (177 ear-tagged on islands, 65 ear-tagged on mainland, 61 radio-collared
and released at point of capture, 32 radio-collared and translocated). In addition
to the width of the water channel, it seems that characteristics such as depth and
flow rate also influence overwater movement of Raccoons.
Raccoons on the Virginia barrier islands are capable of routinely crossing
at least several hundred meters of open water, and they are at least potentially
capable of crossing much greater distances when motivated. Because each of
the islands is within <3 km from the nearest island, this potential for Raccoon
movement complicates predation management. Even more important, at least 12
of the islands—Assawoman, Cedar, Chimney Pole North, Fishermans, Fowling
Point, Holly Bluff, Metompkin, Mockhorn, Raccoon, Skidmore, Smith, and Wallops
(Fig. 1) are potentially within dispersal distance directly from the mainland.
Predation management on these islands is likely to be an on-going activity.
Raccoon abundance on the islands was probably lower in past decades because
of hunting and aggressive game management (Graham 1976a, b), prior to
the designation of the islands for conservation purposes (Byers 1976). Anecdotal
reports of island hunts in local newspapers in the early 1900s suggest that Raccoons
were less abundant in the past (e.g., Peninsula Enterprise, Chincoteague
Notes, 26 October 1912: “Our sportsmen have had lots of fun this week, killing
coons, … In the memory of the oldest inhabitants a coon was never seen on the
Island [Chincoteague] before”). With reduced hunting and human traffic in recent
decades, potential source populations of Raccoons may have increased in
abundance, particularly on the forested islands that offer year-round shelter, thus
increasing the probability of inter-island movement. Increased local abundance
may have increased the probability of movement, leading in turn to increased
probability of occurrence on more islands.
Is the overwater mobility of Raccoons reported here consistent with the notion
that the distribution of Raccoons on the islands has expanded in recent decades,
as proposed by Erwin et al. (2001)? They used information on Raccoon distribution
from Dueser et al. (1979) as a baseline for their proposal. Dueser et al.
(1979) reported Raccoons to be present on only 6 of 11 islands surveyed in 1975
and 1977 (Cobb, Hog, Little Cobb, Parramore, Revel, and Smith). However, this
estimate of Raccoon distribution was probably very conservative because it was
based only on the direct observation of active, free-ranging animals, rather than
on trapped animals, tracks or sign. Erwin et al. (2001), on the other hand, relied
on animal sign such as tracks and dens observed during a single field survey in
1998 to establish the presence of Raccoons on an island. They concluded from
comparison of the records for 1977 and 1998 that Raccoons had spread to at
least 2 additional islands (Wreck and Myrtle) in the interim. It is impossible to
say whether these data represent an expansion in island occupancy over those
21 years or simply reflect different survey techniques in 1998. Nevertheless, our
observations of Raccoon movement reported here suggest that such expansion
might be possible, particularly in conjunction with an overall increase in Raccoon
abundance. Furthermore, Raccoons are now known from repeated system525
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
atic track surveys (R.D. Dueser and N.D. Moncrief, unpubl. data) to occur at
least occasionally on all 5 of the survey islands where active Raccoons were not
reported as being present in 1977 ( Dueser et al. 1979).
This study required relatively large investments of time and funding for
fieldwork and equipment to trap, mark, radio-collar, and monitor individual
animals. Additionally, fieldwork in a barrier island system is further hindered by
logistics related to boat transportation and tidal water-level fluctuations. Given
the economic costs and time required to obtain direct estimates of movement,
we recommend the use of recently developed techniques for indirect estimation
of movement (least-cost path analysis and landscape genetics) as part of future
efforts to investigate overwater movement by Raccoons in this system. Least-cost
path analysis allows researchers to model and visualize functional connectivity
of populations in studies that examine relationships between landscape characteristics
and mobility of organisms (Adriaensen et al. 2003). Landscape genetics
combines spatial data with high-resolution genetic markers to evaluate the role
that landscape variables play in affecting movement of individuals, which is
inferred from genetic diversity and genetic structure of populations (Storfer et
al. 2007). Together with the direct observations of overwater movement we report
in this study, we believe that these newly available methods for indirectly
estimating movement hold great promise for increasing our understanding of the
distribution and dynamics of Raccoon populations in this naturally fragmented
coastal landscape.
Acknowledgments
We thank Frederick Servello and 2 anonymous reviewers for comments that improved
this manuscript. We thank Refuge Manager Sue Rice for permission to work
on Eastern Shore of Virginia National Wildlife Refuge and Skidmore Island, and the
Virginia Department of Game and Inland Fisheries for granting access to Mockhorn
Island Wildlife Management Area. Robert Alonso, Richard Ayres, Eli Fenichel, Sandra
Keil, Scott Kupiec, Erika Miersma, Erika Peterson, Randall Schultz, Jr., Mekbeb Tessema,
and Matthew Wirth assisted with data collection. Brooks Miles Barnes, Director of
Information Services of the Eastern Shore Public Library, kindly called to our attention
the 1912 news report from the Peninsula Enterprise. This work was funded (in part) by
the Virginia Coastal Zone Management Program at the Department of Environmental
Quality through grants NA67OZ0360-01, FY 1996, Task 25; NA77OZ0204-01, FY
1997, Task 1.4; NA87OZ0253-01, FY 1998, Task 1.8; NA97OZ0181-01, FY 1999, Task
1.5; NA17OZ2355-01, FY 2002, Task 12.07; NA03NOS4190104, FY2003, Task 12.10;
NA05NOS4191180, FY 2005, Task 9.06; and NA06NOS4190241, FY 2006, Task 10.06
of the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA), Office of Ocean and
Coastal Resource Management, Under the Coastal Zone Management Act of 1972, as
amended. Views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect
views of NOAA or any of its subagencies. This work also was supported by funding
from NSF grants DEB‑9411974, DEB‑0080381, and DEB-0621014 to the University of
Virginia, the Berryman Institute for Wildlife Damage Management, and the National Fish
and Wildlife Foundation in cooperation with the Disney Company. This is a contribution
from the VCR Long‑Term Ecological Research Program.
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
526
Literature Cited
Adriaensen, F., J.P. Chardon, G. De Blust, E. Swinnen, S. Villalba, H. Gulinck, and E.
Matthysen. 2003. The application of “least-cost” modelling as a functional landscape
model. Landscape and Urban Planning 64:233–247.
Animal Care and Use Committee. 1998. Guidelines for the capture, handling, and care of
mammals as approved by the American Society of Mammalogists. Journal of Mammalogy
79:1416–1431.
Arjo, W.M., C.E. Fisher, J. Armstrong, F. Boyd, and D. Slate. 2007. Effects of natural
barriers and habitat on the western spread of Raccoon rabies in Alabama. Journal of
Wildlife Management 72:1725–1735.
Badger, C.J. 1978. The Atlantic barrier islands: Their preservation benefits all. The Nature
Conservancy News 28:20–23.
Badger, C.J. 1991. Reserve joins shorebird network. The Islands: The Virginia Coast
Reserve. Spring 1991:6.
Badger, C.J. 1997. A generation of coastal conservation. The Islands: The Virginia Coast
Reserve. Special Edition. 23 pp.
Badger, C.J., and R. Kellam. 1989. The Barrier Islands. A Photographic History of Life
on Hog, Cobb, Smith, Cedar, Parramore, Metompkin, and Assateague. Stackpole
Books, Harrisburg, PA. 146 pp.
Bailey, E.P. 1993. Introduction of foxes to Alaskan islands—history, effects on avifauna,
and eradication. US Fish and Wildlife Service, Washington, D.C. Resource Publication
193. 53 pp.
Barnes, B.M., and B.R. Truitt. 1997. Seashore Chronicles: Three Centuries of the Virginia
Barrier Islands. University Press of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA. 248 pp.
Barrier Island Avian Partnership. 1996. Conservation action plan for the avian communities
in the Virginia barrier islands system. Report to the Coastal Zone Management
Program of the Virginia Department of Environmental Quality. 35 pp.
Bigler, W.J., G.L. Hoff, and A.S. Johnson. 1981. Population characteristics of Procyon
lotor marinus in estuarine mangrove swamps of southern Florida. Florida Scientist
44:151–157.
Boettcher, R., T. Penn, R.R. Cross, K.T. Terwilliger, and R.A. Beck. 2007. An overview
of the status and distribution of Piping Plovers in Virginia. Waterbirds 30:138–151.
Brinker, D.F., J.M. McCann, B. Williams, and B.D. Watts. 2007. Colonial-nesting seabirds
in the Chesapeake Bay region: Where have we been and where are we going?
Waterbirds 30: 93–104.
Burger, J., and M. Gochfeld. 1994. Predation and effects of humans on island-nesting
seabirds. Pp. 39–67, In D.N. Nettleship, J. Burger, and M. Gochfeld (Eds.). Seabirds
on Islands: Threats, Case Studies, and Action Plans. Bird Life International Conservation
Series No. 1, Cambridge, UK.
Byers, A.M. 1976. The islands are still there. The Nature Conservancy News 26:9–17.
Dueser, R.D., and W.C. Brown. 1980. Ecological correlates of insular rodent diversity.
Ecology 61:50–56.
Dueser, R. D., W. C. Brown, G. S. Hogue, C. McCaffrey, S. A. McCuskey, and G. J.
Hennessey. 1979. Mammals on the Virginia barrier islands. Journal of Mammalogy,
60:425–429.
Erwin, R.M., B.R. Truitt, and J.E. Jimenez. 2001. Ground-nesting waterbirds and mammalian
carnivores in the Virginia barrier island region: Running out of options. Journal
of Coastal Research 17: 292–296.
527
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
Gehrt, S.D. 2003. Raccoon, Procyon lotor, and allies. Pp. 611–634, In G.A. Feldhamer,
B.C. Thompson, and J.A. Chapman (Eds.). Wild Mammals of North America: Biology
Management and Conservation, 2nd Edition. The Johns Hopkins University Press,
Baltimore, MD. 1216 pp.
Gehrt, S.D., L.B. Fox, and D.L. Spencer. 1993. Locations of Raccoons during flooding
in eastern Kansas. Southwestern Naturalist 38:404–406.
Graham, M.A. 1976a. The Virginia barrier islands: A brief history. The Nature Conservancy
News 26:18–22.
Graham, M.A. 1976b. Land-use history. A study of man's influence on Virginia’s barrier
islands. Pp. 1–86. In R.D. Dueser, M.A. Graham, G.J. Hennessy, C. McCaffrey, A.W.
Niederoda, T.W. Rice, and B. Williams (Eds.). Virginia Coast Reserve Study, Ecosystem
Description. The Nature Conservancy, Arlington, Va. 568 pp.
Grau, G.A., G.C. Sanderson, and J.P. Rogers. 1970. Age determination of Raccoons.
Journal of Wildlife Management 34:364–372.
Hanlon, C.L., D.E. Hayes, A.N. Hamir, D.E. Snyder, S. Jenkins, C.P. Hable, and C.E.
Rupprecht. 1989. Proposed field evaluation of a rabies recombinant vaccine for Raccoons,
(Procyon lotor): Site selection, target species characteristics, and placebo baiting
trials. Journal of Wildlife Diseases 25:555–567.
Hartman, L.H., and D.S. Eastman. 1999. Distribution of introduced Raccoons, Procyon
lotor, on the Queen Charlotte Islands: Implications for burrow-nesting seabirds. Biological
Conservation 88:1–13.
Hayden, B.P., R.D. Dueser, J.T. Callahan, and H.H. Shugart. 1991. Long-term research
at the Virginia coast reserve: Modeling a highly dynamic environment. BioScience
41:310–318.
Kaufmann, J.H. 1990. Raccoon and allies. Pp. 567–585, In J.A. Chapman and G.A. Feldhamer
(Eds). Wild Mammals of North America: Biology, Management, and Economics.
The John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. 1147 pp.
Kauhala, K. 1996. Introduced carnivores in Europe with special reference to central and
northern Europe. Wildlife Biology 2:197–204.
Keišs, O. 2001. Mammalian predator distribution and abundance on the Virginia barrier
islands in relation to breeding habitats of colonial birds. M.Sc. Thesis. Utah State
University, Logan, UT. 88 pp.
McCaffrey, C.A., and R.D. Dueser. 1990. Plant associations on the Virginia barrier islands.
Virginia Journal of Science 41:282–299.
Martin, J.D. 2007. A test of conditioned food aversion to control Raccoon predation on
the eggs of ground-nesting shorebird species on the barrier islands of Virginia. M.Sc.
Thesis. Utah State University, Logan, UT. 122 pp.
Martin, J., A.F. O’Connell, Jr., W.I. Kendall, M.C. Runge, T.R. Simons, A.H. Waldstein,
S.A. Schulte, S.J. Converse, G.W. Smith, T. Pinion, M. Rikard, and E.F. Zipkin. 2010.
Optimal control of native predators. Biological Conservation 143:1751–1758.
Oertel, G.F., M.S. Kearney, S.P. Leatherman, and J.H.Woo. 1989. Anatomy of a barrier
platform: Outer barrier lagoon, Southern Delmarva Peninsula, Virginia. Marine Geology
88:303–328.
Rosatte, R., E. MacDonald, K. Sobey, D. Donovan, L. Bruce, M. Allan, A. Silver, K.
Bennett, L. Brown, K. MacDonald, M. Gibson, T. Buchanan, B. Stevenson, C. Davies,
A. Wandeler, and F. Muldoon. 2007. The elimination of Raccoon rabies from
Wolfe Island, Ontario: Animal density and movements. Journal of Wildlife Diseases
43:242–250.
R.D. Dueser, N.D. Moncrief, O. Keišs, J.D. Martin, J.H. Porter , and B.R. Truitt
2013 Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 20, No. 3
528
Rosatte, R.C., D. Donovan, M. Allan, L. Bruce, T. Buchanan, K. Sobey, B. Stevenson, M.
Gibson, T. MacDonald, M. Whalen, J.C. Davies, F. Muldoon, and A. Wandeler. 2009.
The control of Raccoon rabies in Ontario Canada: Proactive and reactive tactics,
1994–2007. Journal of Wildlife Diseases 45:772–784.
Rosatte, R., M. Ryckman, K. Ing, S. Proceviat, M. Allan, L. Bruce, D. Donovan, and J.C.
Davies. 2010. Density, movements, and survival of Raccoons in Ontario, Canada: Implications
for disease spread and management. Journal of Mammalogy 91:122–135.
Roth, J.D., D.L. Murray, and T.D. Steury. 2008. Spatial dynamics of sympatric canids:
Modeling the impact of Coyote on Red Wolf recovery. Ecological Modeling
214:391–403.
Storfer, A., M.A. Murphy, J.S. Evans, C.S. Goldberg, S. Robinson, S.F. Spear, R. Dezzani,
E. Delmelle, L. Vierling, and L.P. Waits. 2007. Putting the “landscape” in landscape
genetics. Heredity 98:128–142.
Terwilliger, K. (Ed.) 1991. Virginia’s Endangered Species: Proceedings of a Symposium.
McDonald and Woodward Publ. Co., Blacksburg, VA 672 pp.
Waldstein, A. H. 2010. Raccoon ecology and management on Cape Lookout National
Seashore, North Carolina. M.Sc. Thesis. North Carolina State University, Raleigh,
NC. 138 pp.
Wilke A.L., D.F. Brinker, B.D. Watts, A.H. Traut, R. Boettcher, J.M. McCann, B.R. Truitt,
and P.P. Denmon. 2007. American Oystercatchers in Maryland and Virginia, USA:
Status and distribution. Waterbirds 30:152–162.
Williams, B., B. Akers, J.W. Via, and R.A. Beck. 1990. Longitudinal surveys of the beach
nesting and colonial waterbirds of the Virginia barrier islands. Virginia Journal of
Science 41:381–388.
Williams, B., B. Akers, M. Beck, R. Beck, and J. Via. 2005. The 30th annual beachnesting
and colonial waterbirds survey of the Virginia barrier islands— 2004. Raven
76:14–20.
Williams, B., D.F. Brinker, and B.D. Watts. 2007. The status of colonial nesting wading
bird populations within the Chesapeake and Atlantic barrier island-lagoon system.
Waterbirds 30:82–92.
Zar, J.H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis. 4th Edition. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Upper Saddle
River, NJ. 663 pp.