Nest-defense Behavior in Response to the Chatter Call in
Tachycineta bicolor (Tree Swallow)
J. Luke Phillips, Wynn F. Haslam, Alyssa E. Ford, and Richard A. Rowe
Northeastern Naturalist, Volume 26, Issue 1 (2019): 43–51
Full-text pdf (Accessible only to subscribers. To subscribe click here.)
Access Journal Content
Open access browsing of table of contents and abstract pages. Full text pdfs available for download for subscribers.
Current Issue: Vol. 30 (3)
Check out NENA's latest Monograph:
Monograph 22
Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 26, No. 1
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019
43
2019 NORTHEASTERN NATURALIST 26(1):43–51
Nest-defense Behavior in Response to the Chatter Call in
Tachycineta bicolor (Tree Swallow)
J. Luke Phillips1,2, Wynn F. Haslam1,3, Alyssa E. Ford1,4, and Richard A. Rowe1,*
Abstract - Birds produce a variety of vocalizations ranging in function from attracting a
mate to alerting others of danger. One vocalization of Tachycineta bicolor (Tree Swallow),
the chatter call, is reported to serve as both a mate-attraction call and a nest-defense call.
We used playback techniques to investigate the nest-defense behavior of Tree Swallows to
calls of Toxostoma rufum (Brown Thrasher; control) and Sialia sialis (Eastern Bluebird;
nest competitor), and Tree Swallow shriek (alarm call) and chatter calls. We measured time
spent foraging, remaining in the nest box, and on nest defense. Tree Swallows spent 80%
of their time on nest defense in response to the chatter call. The chatter-call and shriek-call
nest-defense responses were nearly identical. Our data show that the chatter call functions
as an alarm call and elicits nest-defense behavior.
Introduction
Cohen (1978) first described the vocalizations of Tachycineta bicolor (Vielliot)
(Tree Swallow) and presented 9 distinct calls. Others have subsequently supported
his descriptions (Leonard et al. 1997, Robertson et al. 1992, Sharman et al. 1994,
Winkler 1992). Two of the calls, the shriek and chatter calls, are associated with
nest defense, and the chatter call could function to attract mates to the nest (Sharman
et al. 1994). Thus, the chatter call appears to carry a message stating either
“come here” to potential mates or “go away” to potential competitors; but how the
chatter call is modified to relay this message or if it can convey both messages at
the same time has not be elucidated.
The shriek call is an alarm call directed towards predators near the nest or when
attacking a predator, and on occasion, to conspecifics near the nest or during chases
(Cohen 1978, Sharman et al. 1994, Winkler et al. 2011). The shriek call, as described
by Sharman et al. (1994), is given when perched or in flight and in response
to a predator or conspecific flying too close to the nest (Winkler et al. 2011). The
shriek call functions to alert a mate or offspring to the presence of a threat and to
recruit other Tree Swallows to aid in defense (Curio 1978, Winkler 1994, Winkler
et al. 2011).
The role of the chatter call in Tree Swallow communication is unclear because it
is used in various behavioral contexts. Similar to the shriek call, the chatter call is
given near the nest box when a conspecific approaches, and it is most common during
the time prior to egg-laying (Cohen 1978, 1989; Robertson et al. 1992; Winkler
1Department of Biology, Virginia Military Institute, Lexington, VA 24450. 2Current address
- 2800 Shadow Lake Road, Blacksburg, VA 24060. 3Current address - 361 Bradford Road,
Mars Hill, NC, 28754. 4Current address - 3304 8th Street NE, Minot, ND 58703. *Corresponding
author - rowera@vmi.edu.
Manuscript Editor: Daniel Keppie
Northeastern Naturalist
44
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019 Vol. 26, No. 1
et al. 2011). The chatter call can also function as a mate-attraction call (Robertson
et al. 1992). This call is described as being a nest-site advertising call and does not
appear to be associated with defending a mate from potential extra-pair copulations
(Sharman et al. 1994). The chatter call is incorporated into the dawn song and day
song of Tree Swallows and is used in combination with other calls in these mateattraction
and territorial songs (Horn 1996, Taft 2011). Hence, the chatter call may
be given in response to conspecifics intruding into nesting space, but there is no
direct evidence that it elicits nest-defense behavior from residents in response to a
conspecific or predator intruding into a territory.
Our goal was to investigate the role of the chatter call in nest defense. We used
playback techniques to assess whether: (1) the chatter call elicits nest defense
behavior; (2) the response to the chatter call is similar to the shriek call (a wellestablished
predator-alarm call: Winkler 1992, Winkler et al. 2011); and (3) the
song of a known nest competitor and potential nest usurper, Sialia sialis (L.) (Eastern
Bluebird) (Finch 1990; Rowe and Phillips 2016; Tuttle 1987, 1991), near a nest
elicits nest-defense behavior.
Methods
Study site
We conducted this study during the 2013–2016 breeding seasons at the Virginia
Military Institute Biology Department’s Field Research Site (37°46'45.09"N,
79°23'30.51"W) ~4 km east of Lexington in west-central Virginia. The site is a 9-ha
hayfield with 2 small thickets and fence rows and mature trees separating the field
site from adjacent farmland. In 2002, we placed 30 nest boxes (14 cm x 14 cm x
25 cm, with an opening 16.5 cm above floor) 1.5–1.8 m above the ground in a grid
on the hayfield to attract Tree Swallows. We mounted all nest boxes on posts with
anti-predator baffles (stove-pipe, 20.5 cm x 61 cm). The mean distance between
nest boxes was 46.2 m.
Field methods
In west-central Virginia, most Tree Swallows arrive in mid-March (R.A. Rowe,
pers. observ.) and begin nest construction in mid-April; first-egg date varies from 27
April to 13 May (R.A. Rowe, unpubl. data). We checked nest-building status weekly
in March and twice weekly in the first 2 weeks of April until complete nest cups were
present. We monitored nests every other day until feathers were present in the nest
cup (indicating that egg laying was imminent). We examined individual nests daily
during egg laying and at the expected time of hatching to establish onset of incubation
(we defined day 0 of incubation as the first day when no new egg was laid) and
the nestling phase. We defined nest day (ND) 0 of the nestling phase as the first day
a nestling was found in the nest. When clutches were complete or the nestlings had
hatched, we checked nest boxes every other day to ensure the eggs or nestlings were
present. We emptied all nest boxes in late February, prior to the onset of the nesting
season. The study was approved by the Animal Subjects Committee at the Virginia
Military Institute. None of the birds in this study were marked.
Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 26, No. 1
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019
45
Playback protocol
We used playback tapes of Tree Swallow vocalizations to elicit nest-defense
behaviors from resident pairs of Tree Swallows. Each playback tape consisted
of 5 min of silence followed by 5 min of playback scenario. We used 4 different
playback scenarios to investigate behavioral responses to the chatter call. We used
the typical singing of Toxostoma rufum (L.) (Brown Thrasher) as a control for the
presence of the tape recorder, tripod, and the presentation of sounds at the nest
box. Brown Thrashers were present at our field site but do not present a threat to
Tree Swallows because they are not cavity nesters and are too large to enter a nest
box. During the 2016 nesting season, we used playback of an Eastern Bluebird
singing to investigate whether the presence of a potential nest competitor that was
not a predator would elicit nest-defense behavior. We employed both the Brown
Thrasher and Eastern Bluebird calls with permission from the Borror Laboratory
of Bioacoustics, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH (Brown Thrasher
BLB 4508; Eastern Bluebird BLB 17538). We also played chatter and shriek
calls to provide a comparison of the chatter call to a known alarm call. I recorded
both the chatter and shriek calls during 2013 at the research site using a Telinga
Pro parabolic dish (Tekinga, Tobo, Sweden), Sennhieser microphone (MKH 20;
Wedemark, Germany), and a Fostex FR-2 digital field recorder (Fostex, Wiltshire,
UK). I created playback loops for all tapes using Raven Pro 1.4 (Bioacoustics Research
Program, The Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY).
We presented playback tapes at each nest box during each nesting phase, incubation
(day 7 or 8 after the last egg was laid), young nestlings (ND 4 or 5), and
older nestlings (ND 12 or 13). Over the years of this study, the mean length for
incubation was 13.5 d at our field site (n = 62), and fledging occurred at ~ND 19
(n = 38). The order of presentation of playback tapes was varied so that each playback
was played at a nest during the nesting period (incubation, young nestlings,
or older nestling) but no nest was tested with the same playback twice. During the
2013–2015 nesting seasons, the 3 playback tapes were the Brown Thrasher singing,
shriek call, and chatter-call tapes. During the 2014–2015 nesting seasons, we documented
suspected nest-usurpation by Eastern Bluebirds (Rowe and Phillips 2016).
As a result, during the 2016 nesting season, we substituted playback of a singing
Eastern Bluebird for the shriek-call playback in order to investigate whether Tree
Swallows would respond to the presence of a potential nest competitor or usurper
near the nest box.
We conducted all playback trials between 0700 and 0930. During playback trials,
we placed the tape recorder on a tripod ~1 m in front of a nest box and 1.5 m
above ground level; 2 observers were positioned ~30 m from the focal nest box and
30 m from each other. An initial 5-min silent period allowed the focal pair to adjust
to the presence of the tape player and tripod and to allow the observers to locate the
focal pair for observations during the playback trial. During a playback trial, observers
used a hand-held voice recorder (Olympus WS-822 digital voice recorder)
to verbally document the behavior of the birds. We employed Raven Pro 1.4 to
analyze these recordings for time spent performing each behavior. We calculated
the time spent for each behavior as the average from both observers.
Northeastern Naturalist
46
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019 Vol. 26, No. 1
Behaviors
We noted the following behaviors during playback sessions: foraging, perched,
in the nest box, and circling. We combined active feeding, flying in a direct line
above the grass, or flying out of sight (away from the nest box) as foraging. We
defined perched as being perched on or within 10 m of the nest box. The “in the nest
box” category occurred when either or both birds (a rare event) were known to be
in the nest box and, thus, we could not observe their actual behavior. Our initial observations
during playback trials in 2013 revealed that Tree Swallows left the nest
almost immediately (within the first 30 sec) after either the chatter- or shriek-call
playbacks began. As a result, we decided not to include “in the nest box” as a nestdefense
behavior. We defined circling as flying that was centered around the nest
box. For the analyses in the present study, nest defense was the sum of the amount
of time spent circling and perched on/near the nest box.
During the 2016 nesting season, we recorded vocalizations from male Tree
Swallows perched on their nest box during the egg-laying and incubation phases.
These vocalizations were recorded using a Telinga Pro parabolic dish, a Sennhieser
microphone (MKH 20), and a Fostex FR-2 digital field recorder. From these recordings,
we were able to identify 11 cases in of a male singing from his nest box when
the location of his mate was known and an intruding conspecific Tree Swallow flew
into/through the territory. We analyzed these 11 recordings using Raven Pro 1.4
(Bioacoustics Research Program, The Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology) to investigate
if the rate or the percentage of chatter calls changed when a conspecific was
present in the territory.
Statistical analysis
We converted data collected on time spent performing each behavior to percentage
of time spent during a playback session. We transformed data to the
arcsine square-root of the percent for statistical analysis (Sokal and Rolf 1995).
As a means to validate our methods, we used the general linear model ANOVA
(threshold for significance P ≤ 0.05) and a Tukey post-hoc test (in Minitab 17.3.1)
to determine if there was an effect (increase in behavioral responses) on subsequent
Brown Thrasher playback trials by playing a shriek or chatter call first. We
found no effect of playing a shriek or chatter call first on the behavioral responses
in subsequent playback trials across the nesting phases (n = 48 for all ANOVAs;
foraging F2 = 1.62, P = 0.21; in the nest box F2 = 2.03, P = 0.138; circling F2 =
0.48, P = 0.62; nest defense F2 = 1.68, P = 0.19). Thus, we are confident that our
experimental procedure did not sensitize the resident pair and elicit an enhanced
response during future trials. We conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA and
Tukey post-hoc test to determine if mean differences in time spent performing
behaviors in response to the playbacks at each nest phases (R Core Team 2018)
were meaningful. We used a paired t-test to evaluate changes in the number and
percentage of chatter calls given prior to and during the intrusion of a conspecific
into a territory (Minitab 17.3.1).
Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 26, No. 1
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019
47
Results
Brown Thrasher playback trials
In response to the Brown Thrasher, the mean percentage of time Tree Swallows
spent performing the different behaviors (Table 1) during the 3 nesting phases
(incubation, young nestlings = ND 4–5, old nestlings = ND 12–13) did not differ.
As expected, the amount of time spent in the nest box (Table 1) decreased at ND
12–13, when both adults were foraging and nestlings had attained adult weight
(R.A. Rowe, unpubl. data). It should be noted that only female Tree Swallows incubate
the eggs and thus, the mean values are indicative of only the females’ behavior.
During Brown Thrasher playback trials at all nesting phases, Tree Swalllow males
spent less than 3% of their time in the nest box. The time spent on nest defense showed
no significant changes as the nest progressed in age, thus, the time spent on this
behavior was consistent (Table 1).
Chatter and shriek call playback trials
Our comparison of the mean time spent performing different behaviors in response
to both the chatter and shriek calls (Table 2) revealed that there were no
Table 2. Mean percent time (mean ± SD) spent by nesting Tree Swallow pairs in response to playback
of the chatter-call and shriek-call tapes. Nest defense is the sum of time spent circling and perched
during a trial. We conducted statistical analyses using a repeated-measures ANOVA (Fnumdf,dendf) with
P-values from a Tukey post-hoc comparison of means. Means with different letters in each column
are different at ≤ 0.05.
% time spent on behavioral responses
Playback n Foraging In nest box Nest defense
Brown Thrasher 134 58.0 ± 40.8A 21.6 ± 8.1 A 20.4 ± 31.0 A
Bluebird 52 45.6 ± 39.9 A 11.3 ± 27.9 AB 43.2 ± 38.2A
Chatter call 134 18.2 ± 25.6B 2.9 ± 14.0B 79.2 ± 27.4B
Shriek call 40 16.4 ± 16.6B 0.08 ± 4.3B 82.9 ± 17.5B
Statistics F3,74 = 27.29 F3,74 = 13.55 F3,74 = 67.65
P = 0.01 P = 0.01 P = 0.01
Table 1. Mean percent time (mean ± SD) spent foraging, in the nest box, and on nest defense by nesting
pairs of Tree Swallows during Brown Thrasher playback trials. The Brown Thrasher tape served
as a control in our study; the data represent behaviors of Tree Swallows in response to the playback
protocol. Nest defense is the sum of time spent circling and perched during a trial. We conducted statistical
analyses using a repeated-measures ANOVA (Fnumdf,dendf) with P-values from a Tukey post-hoc
comparison of means. Means with different letters in each column are dif ferent at ≤0.05.
% time spent on behavioral responses
Playback n Foraging In nest box Nest defense
Incubation 56 49.4 ± 44.5A 24.6 ± 42.3A 26.1 ± 36.5A
ND 4–5 50 59.4 ± 38.9A 25.4 ± 38.4A 15.2 ± 26.2A
ND 12–13 28 73.0 ± 32.1A 8.6 ± 24.4B 18.3 ± 25.5A
Statistics F2,74 = 3.68 F2,74 = 4.59 F2,74 =2.15
P < 0.03 P < 0.01 P = 0.12
Northeastern Naturalist
48
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019 Vol. 26, No. 1
significant differences in time spent foraging, in the nest box, or on nest defense
(P = 0.07–1.0, Tukey comparisons). Both the chatter and shriek calls elicited similar
responses when compared to the Brown Thrasher playback. The time spent foraging,
in the nest box, or on nest defense in response to either the chatter call or shriek call
was different (P < 0.01 for all comparisons) from the Brown Thrasher playback.
Eastern Bluebird playback trials
During the 2016 nesting season, we investigated if the presence of an Eastern
Bluebird singing near the nest box would elicit any changes in behaviors by Tree
Swallows in response to the Brown Thrasher, Eastern Bluebird, and chatter call
playbacks. A comparison of the Eastern Bluebird and Thrasher playbacks showed
no differences in Tree Swallow behavior as nests progressed from incubation
through ND 12–13 (F 2,74, for phase-effect, P values from Tukey: F = 0.12–2.16, P =
0.13–0.89). In addition, there were no meaningful differences in the amount of time
spent foraging, in the nest box, or on nest defense when the Eastern Bluebird and
Brown Thrasher tapes were compared (F 3,74 [see Table 2 for repeated-measures F
values for each behavior], P = 0.25–0.99). We compared the Tree Swallow responses
to Eastern Bluebird and chatter-call playbacks to examine if a singing Eastern
Bluebird elicited behavioral changes in the Tree Swallows. There were differences
in the responses for nest defense and foraging (P values from Tukey comparison of
means: foraging P = 0.01, circling P = 0.01, nest defense P = 0.01) but no differences
in the time spent in the nest box (perched P =1.00, in the nest box P = 0.23).
Chatter calls in response to intruding Tree Swallow
During 2016, we recorded 11 situations in which a male Tree Swallow was vocalizing
while perched on a nest box and, subsequently, another Tree Swallow flew
into the territory. We compared the number and percentage of chatter calls given
prior to the intrusion and during the time the conspecific was within the territory.
The mean number of chatter calls increased significantly (paired t-test: t = 4.06, P =
0.01, n = 11) from 6.2 ± 0.8 (mean ± SD) chatters per second prior to the intrusion
to 9.3 ± 0.5 chatters per second during the time the conspecific was present. During
the time prior to the intrusion, chatter calls constituted 78.3% ± 6.7 of the vocalizations,
and 95.9% ± 2.2 during the intrusion (paired t-test: t = 2.46, P = 0.03, n = 11).
Discussion
Our data demonstrates that the chatter call primarily elicits nest-defense responses
and increases nest defense by 4-fold, that the percentage of time spent on
nest defense is similar to the response elicited by the shriek call (constituting ~80%
of the swallows’ time, Table 2), and that the responses to the chatter call differ from
those to Brown Thrasher or Eastern Bluebird playback trials. Both the chatter and
shriek calls elicit similar nest-defense behavior; thus, we speculate that Tree Swallows
have 2 alarm calls to provide additional information about the threat. We note
that the data presented in the present study do not rule out the possibility that the
chatter call also serves to attract mates as suggested by Sharman et al. (1994).
Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 26, No. 1
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019
49
Eastern Bluebirds pose a threat because they are known to usurp the nests of
other species, including Tree Swallows (Frye and Rogers 2004; Gowaty and Plissner
2015; Rowe and Phillips 2016; Tuttle 1987, 1991). Eastern Bluebirds have been
documented to usurp nests during the building, egg-laying, incubation, and the
early nestling phases. Loss of a nest due to usurpation or nest parasitism results in
a loss of reproductive effort, which is especially critical in Tree Swallows, which
only produce 1 clutch per year (Leffelaar and Robertson 1985). Thus, the reproductive
value of each clutch is high in Tree Swallows, and possessing a specific call,
such as the chatter call, that can alert a mate of the presence of a nest competitor
can be important.
The responses of Tree Swallows to chatter-call playbacks showed changes in the
behaviors that we monitored when compared to the Brown Thrasher (control). The
increase in nest defense (the sum of circling and perched behaviors in this study)
in response to the chatter-call playback provides the opportunity to be seen by the
threatening bird and to monitor the activity of the threat. Curio (1978) notes that
alarm calls serve to notify the predator that it is seen and thus, the element of surprise
is lost. This visibility during nest defense performs some of the functions of
an alarm. Tree Swallows commonly fly in response to a threat (Winkler et al. 2011),
which provides an immediate avenue for escape, allows the bird to visually locate
the threat, and allows the swallow to monitor the threat’s activity. We describe the
Tree Swallows’ nest-defense behavior as being vigilant or patrolling the territory
in response to the potential presence of a threat to the nest.
Some researchers have suggested that nest defense should increase with the
increasing age of a nest and thus, the inherent value of the nestlings (Forbes et al.
1994, Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988). However, Tree Swallows do not appear
to increase their nest-defense behaviors as nests progress from eggs through
older nestling stages (Table 1; Hainstock et al. 2010, Lombardo 1991, Winkler
1991, present study).
Although both the chatter and shriek calls have an alarm-call function, they
may serve additional functions in the vocal repertoire. Our data on nest defense in
response to the shriek call are consistent with other studies; however, the possibility
that the chatter call serves multiple functions is likely (Sharman et al. 1994,
Winkler et al. 2011). Referential-based alarm-call systems have been described
in only a few species of birds. Templeton et al. (2005) first described encoding of
information into alarm calls by Parus atricapillus (L.) (Black-capped Chickadee)
and the Baeolophus bicolor (L.) (Tufted Titmouse). These species varied the notes
within their alarm calls to encode information about the type of threat present. Our
data suggest the possibility that one use of the chatter call of Tree Swallows is to
broadcast the presence of a threat from a nest competitor or usurper, of the same or
different species, that has entered the territory, and to alert an intruding Tree Swallow
that it has been seen. The increase in chatter calls during intrusion events that
we documented could serve both of these functions. Nest usurpation is found in a
number of species (Lindell 1996) and is common in Tree Swallows (Finch 1990;
Frye and Rogers 2004; Haslam et al. 2016; Hersey 1933; Rowe and Phillips 2016;
Tuttle 1987, 1991).
Northeastern Naturalist
50
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019 Vol. 26, No. 1
Our study shows that the chatter call elicits responses that are similar to the
shriek call, which is an alarm call; however, it would be necessary to present threats
(predators or competitors) to the nest and record the vocalizations of Tree Swallows
in response to these threats in order to confirm the chatter call’s role as an alarm
call. Tree Swallows are a nest-cavity–limited species that will readily occupy nest
boxes. As a result, large aggregations of Tree Swallows are possible along Eastern
Bluebird trails and nest-box grids. The increase in availability of nest boxes associated
with Eastern Bluebird conservation has resulted in an expansion of their
breeding range (Winkler et al 2011) and has created large, semi-colonial breeding
groups. In these large breeding assemblages, it is common for shriek calls to recruit
individuals from neighboring nests to assist in harassing a predator (Winkler 1994).
We did not specifically monitor recruits to our playback trials, and could provide
valuable insights to compare the effect of the shriek call and chatter call in attracting
recruits.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by funds from the Virginia Military Institute’s (VMI) Summer
Undergraduate Research Initiative, the Swope Summer Scholars Program, the VMI Biology
Department, and the North American Bluebird Society. This manuscript benefitted greatly
from comments provided by A. Alerding and P. Moosman and 4 anonymous reviewers. We
thank R. Humston for his assistance with the statistical analyses and the use of the R statistical
software.
Literature Cited
Cohen, R.R. 1978. Vocalizations of the Tree Swallow. (Abstract). Journal of the Colorado-
Wyoming Academy of Science 10:50.
Cohen, R.R. 1989. Day-song of the Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor). (Abstract). Journal
of the Colorado Wyoming Academy of Science 21:11.
Curio, E. 1978. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. I. Telenomic hypotheses and
predictions. Zietschrief Tierpsychology 53:139–152.
Finch, D. 1990. Effects of predation and competitor interference on nesting success of
House Wrens and Tree Swallows. Condor 92:674–687.
Forbes, M.R.L., R.G. Clark, P.J. Weatherhead, and T. Armstrong. 1994. Risk-taking by
female ducks: Intra- and interspecific tests of nest-defense theory. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology 34:79–85.
Frye, G.G., and K.K. Rogers. 2004. Probable cavity usurpation via interspecific killing by
the Mountain Bluebird (Sialia currucoides). Northwestern Naturalist 85:126–128.
Gowaty, P.A., and J.H. Plissner. 2015. Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis), version 2.0. No. 381, In
A.F. Poole (Ed.). The Birds of North America OnLine. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca,
NY. Available onlne at https://doi.org/10.2173/bna.381. Accessed 21 April 2018.
Hainstock, M.H., M.C Smith, J. Carr, and D. Shutler. 2010. Parental investment and brood
value in Tree Swallows, Tachycineta bicolor. Behaviour 147:441–464.
Haslam, W.F., R.A. Rowe, and J.L. Phillips. 2016. A mixed brood following usurpation of a
Carolina Chickadee nest by Tree Swallows. Southeastern Naturalist 15:44–49.
Hersey, F.S. 1933. Notes on Tree Swallows and Bluebirds. Auk 50:109–110.
Northeastern Naturalist Vol. 26, No. 1
J.L. Phillips, W.. Haslam, A.E. Ford, and R.A. Rowe
2019
51
Horn, A.G. 1996. Dawn-song repertoires of Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Canadian
Journal of Zoology 79:1084–1091.
Leffelaar, D., and R.J. Robertson. 1985. Nest usurpation and female competition for breeding
opportunities by Tree Swallows. Wilson Bulletin 97:221–224.
Leonard, M.L., N. Fernandez, and G. Brown. 1997. Parental calls and nestling behavior in
Tree Swallows. Auk 114:668–672.
Lindell, C. 1996. Patterns of nest usurpation: When should species converge on nest niches?
Condor 98:464–473.
Lombardo, M.P. 1991. Sexual differences in parental effort during the nestling period in
Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Auk 108:393–404.
Montgomerie, R.D., and P.J. Weatherhead. 1988. Risks and rewards of nest defense by parent
birds. Quarterly Review of Biology 63:167–187.
R Core Team. 2018. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Version 3.5.0.
R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. Available online at https://
www.R-project.org. Accessed 1 May 2018.
Robertson, R.J., B.J. Skutchbury, and R.R. Cohen. 1992. Tree Swallow. No. 11, In A. Poole,
P. Stettenheim, and F. Gill (Eds.). The Birds of North America. The Academy of Natural
Sciences, Philadelphia, PA, and The American Ornithologists’ Union, Washington, DC.
Rowe, R.A., and J.L. Phillips. 2016. Suspected usurpation of Tree Swallow nests by Eastern
Bluebirds in west-central Virginia. Northeastern Naturalist 23:482–489.
Sharman, M.Y., R.J. Robertson, and L.M. Ratcliffe. 1994. Vocalizations of the Tree Swallow
(Tachycineta bicolor) during the prelaying period: A structural and contextual analysis.
American Midland Naturalist 32:264–274.
Sokal, R.R., and F.J. Rohlf. 1995. Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in
Biological Research, 3rd Edition. W.H. Freeman, New York, NY. 776 pp.
Taft, B.N. 2011. The role of dawn song in Tree Swallows and its place in the diversity of
oscine song learning. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst, MA.
125 pp.
Templeton, C.N., E. Greene, and K. Davis. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: Black-Capped
Chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937.
Tuttle, R.M. 1987. A six-year study of nesting Tree Swallows in Delaware State Park, Delaware,
Ohio 1979-1984. Sialia 9:3–7, 34.
Tuttle, R.M. 1991. An analysis of the interspecific competition of Eastern Bluebirds, Tree
Swallows, and House Wrens in Delaware State Park, Delaware, Ohio, 1979–1986. Sialia
13:3–13.
Winkler, D.W. 1991. Parental-investment decision-rules in Tree Swallows: Parental defense,
abandonment, and the so-called Concorde Fallacy. Behavioral Ecology 2:133–142.
Winkler, D.W. 1992. Causes and consequences of variation in parental defense behavior by
Tree Swallows. Condor 94:502–520.
Winkler, D.W. 1994. Anti-predator defense by neighbors as a responsive amplifier of parental
defense in Tree Swallows. Animal Behaviour 47:595–605.
Winkler, D.W., K.K. Hallinger, D.R. Ardia, R.J. Robertson, B.J. Stutchbury, and R.R. Cohen.
2011. Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor), version 2.0. No. 11, In A.F. Poole (Ed.).
The Birds of North America. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY. Available online
at https://doi.org/10.2173/bna.11 Accessed 25 April 2018.